Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jun 17;27(1):44.
doi: 10.1007/s10071-024-01881-7.

This or not that: select and reject control of relational responding in rats using a blank comparison procedure with odor stimuli

Affiliations

This or not that: select and reject control of relational responding in rats using a blank comparison procedure with odor stimuli

Bobbie Faith Wolff et al. Anim Cogn. .

Abstract

The blank comparison (BLC) task was developed to assess stimulus relations in discrimination learning; that is, are subjects learning to "select" the correct stimulus (S+) or "reject" the incorrect stimulus (S-) or both? This task has been used to study exclusion learning, mostly in humans and monkeys, and the present study extends the procedure to rats. The BLC task uses an ambiguous stimulus (BLC+/-) that replaces S+ (in the presence of S-) and replaces S- (in the presence of S+). In the current experiment, four rats were trained to remove session-novel scented lids from sand-filled cups in a two-choice, simultaneous presentation procedure called the Odor Span Task (OST) before being trained on the BLC procedure using odors as the discriminative stimuli. The BLC training procedure utilized simple discrimination training (S+ and S-) and added select (S+ and BLC-) and reject (BLC+ and S-) trial types. All rats demonstrated accurate performance in sessions with both select and reject type trials. Next, BLC probe trials were interspersed in standard OST sessions to assess the form of stimulus control in the OST. Rats performed accurately on select type probe trials (similar to baseline OST performance) and also showed above chance accuracy on reject type trials. Thus, we demonstrated that rats could acquire an odor-based version of the BLC task and that both select and exclusion-based (reject) relations were active in the OST. The finding of exclusion in rats under the rigorous BLC task conditions confirms that exclusion-based responding is not limited to humans and non-human primates.

Keywords: Blank comparison; Exclusion learning; Rats; Reject control; Select control; Stimulus control topography.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflicts of interest or competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Arena testing apparatus. Note In the testing arena, there are two circular arrays (rings) in which the stimuli can be placed. In the outer ring, there are 12 equidistant holes around the perimeter. Note that a cup with lid is in the 4 o’clock position. There are six equidistant holes in the center ring; there is a cup with lid in the 6 o’clock position
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Percent correct responses by each subject in blocked reject, select, and interspersed select trials in the last five sessions of BLC training. Note Error bars are standard error of the mean. Dashed line indicates chance responding. All means significantly above chance (p < .001, binomial test)
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Percent correct responding by each subject on OST and SD sessions during BLC training. Note Error bars are standard error of the mean. Dashed line indicates chance responding. All means significantly above chance (p < .001, binomial test)
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Precent correct responding by each subject on OST, select, and reject trials across all 25 BLC in OST sessions. Note Error bars are standard error of the mean. Dashed line indicates chance responding. All means significantly above chance (p < .001, binomial test)

Similar articles

References

    1. April LB, Bruce K, Galizio M (2013) The magic number 70 (plus or minus 20): variables determining performance in the rodent odor span task. Learn Motiv 44:143–158. 10.1016/j.lmot.2013.03.001 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Aust U, Range F, Steurer M, Huber L (2008) Inferential reasoning by exclusion in pigeons, dogs, and humans. Anim Cogn 11(4):587–597. 10.1007/s10071-008-0149-0 - PubMed
    1. Beran M, Washburn D (2002) Chimpanzee responding during conditional matching-to-sample: control by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 78(3):497–508. 10.1901/jeab.2002.78-497 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Biolsi KL, Woo KL (2022) Equivalence classification, learning by exclusion, and long-term memory in pinnipeds: cognitive mechanisms demonstrated through research with subjects under human care and in the field. Anim Cogn 25(5):1077–1090. 10.1007/s10071-022-01658-w - PubMed
    1. Clement ST, Zentall RT (2003) Choice based on exclusion in pigeons. Psychon Bull Rev 10(4):959–964. 10.3758/bf03196558 - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources