Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
[Preprint]. 2024 Jun 6:2024.06.04.596708.
doi: 10.1101/2024.06.04.596708.

A paradoxical misperception of relative motion

Affiliations

A paradoxical misperception of relative motion

Josephine C D'Angelo et al. bioRxiv. .

Update in

  • A paradoxical misperception of relative motion.
    D'Angelo JC, Tiruveedhula P, Weber RJ, Arathorn DW, Roorda A. D'Angelo JC, et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024 Nov 26;121(48):e2410755121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2410755121. Epub 2024 Nov 21. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024. PMID: 39570307 Free PMC article.

Abstract

Motion perception is considered a hyperacuity. The presence of a visual frame of reference to compute relative motion is necessary to achieve this sensitivity [Legge, Gordon E., and F. W. Campbell. "Displacement detection in human vision." Vision Research 21.2 (1981): 205-213.]. However, there is a special condition where humans are unable to accurately detect relative motion: images moving in a direction consistent with retinal slip where the motion is unnaturally amplified can, under some conditions, appear stable [Arathorn, David W., et al. "How the unstable eye sees a stable and moving world." Journal of Vision 13.10.22 (2013)]. In this study, we asked: Is world-fixed retinal image background content necessary for the visual system to compute the direction of eye motion to render in the percept images moving with amplified slip as stable? Or, are non-visual cues sufficient? Subjects adjusted the parameters of a stimulus moving in a random trajectory to match the perceived motion of images moving contingent to the retina. Experiments were done with and without retinal image background content. The perceived motion of stimuli moving with amplified retinal slip was suppressed in the presence of visual content; however, higher magnitudes of motion were perceived under conditions with no visual cues. Our results demonstrate that the presence of retinal image background content is essential for the visual system to compute its direction of motion. The visual content that might be thought to provide a strong frame of reference to detect amplified retinal slips, instead paradoxically drives the misperception of relative motion.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Author comments. Declaration of interests. Austin Roorda and Pavan Tiruveedhula are co-inventors on US Patent 10130253, assigned to the University of California.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Display configuration. (a) The projector drew a fixation target and the surrounding 17° field displayed either white light or a binarized noise pattern. The AOSLO generated the stimuli positioned 2° temporally. These images were simultaneously projected onto the retina through a beamsplitter. (b) Three stimuli conditions: a Gain −1.5 stimulus moves with increased retinal slip, a Gain +1.5 stimulus moves in advance of retinal motion, and a Gain 0 stimulus is world-fixed. Undergoing the same retinal motion, a Gain −1.5 stimulus moves 2.5x more on the retina than a Gain 0 stimulus, and a Gain 0 stimulus moves 2x more on the retina than a Gain +1.5 stimulus. (c) Simulated trajectories showing uncorrelated, Brownian motion (left) and positively correlated, persistent motion (right). The red dot indicates the starting position for each trace. Both paths were generated from the same average step length, L = 0.6 arcminutes, and have similar speeds (S), yet have different diffusion constants (D) due to different α values. Full descriptions of these parameters are in Materials and Methods. (d-e) Experiment sequence. Subjects fixated on the target and attended to the stimuli positioned 2° temporally. The retina-contingent stimulus moved contingent to each subject’s idiosyncratic fixational eye motion. The random walk stimulus moved independent to eye motion, with pre-generated random offsets. Under background-present conditions (d), the projector field displayed a binarized noise pattern which changed after each presentation and the fixation target remained on for the entire duration. Under background-absent conditions (e), the “Ganzfeld effect” was achieved by setting up a white paper with an aperture in front of the display permitting only the AOSLO and projector beams to enter the eye, shown in (a). LEDs were taped around the eye to illuminate the paper and the luminance was adjusted so that the subject saw only the stimuli in a white full-field surround. Owing to its proximity to the eye, the natural blur of the aperture rendered the transition between the display and luminance-matched paper invisible. The fixation target was timed to turned off when the stimuli were presented.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Individual results from six subjects tested under background-present and background-absent conditions. The conditions are indicated by labels on the top left corner of each graph. The small circles are the six perceptual matches for Gain −1.5 stimuli (blue open symbols), Gain +1.5 stimuli (green filled symbols), and Gain 0 stimuli (yellow filled symbols). The large circles are the averages of the matches with standard error of the mean bars. The red arrows show the extent to which the αWM deviated from Brownian motion. Arrows pointing right indicate αWM>1, arrows pointing left indicate αWM<1, and no arrow means that αWM=1+/0.02. Longer arrows correspond to higher deviations from Brownian motion. The arrow length in the legend indicates pure persistence (αWM=2) if pointing right or pure antipersistence (αWM=0) if pointing left.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Computed ratios from six subjects tested under background-present and background-absent conditions. The red arrows show the extent to which the αWM deviated from Brownian motion. Arrows pointing up and right indicate αWM>1, arrows pointing left indicate αWM<1, and no arrow means that αWM=1+/0.02. Longer arrows correspond to higher deviations from Brownian motion. The arrow length in the legend indicates pure persistence αWM=2) if pointing right or pure antipersistence (αWM=0) if pointing up/right or pure antipersistence if pointing down/left (αWM=0).
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
a-c Violin plots (9) showing the distribution of the (a) DEM, the (b) αEM, and (c) SEM for three Gains (−1.5, +1.5, and 0) under two conditions (background-present and background-absent). In the center of the violin, the white circle represents the median and the dark bars represent the interquartile range. The black points are the (a) mean DEM, (b) mean αEM, and (c) mean SEM for each subject across their six respective trials for each Gain. The asterisks indicate statistical significance (p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively) from a post hoc Tukey-Kramer test following a two-factor repeated-measures ANOVA.
Fig. 5.
Fig. 5.
Example gaze traces for subject 10003L during Gain −1.5 retina-contingent presentations under (c) background-present (1500-ms duration and αEM=0.90) and (d) background-absent (750-ms duration and αEM=1.29) conditions. The red dot indicates the starting position for each trace. The gaze directions are labeled: left (L), right (R), up (U), and down (D).
Fig. 6.
Fig. 6.
Computed ratios from six subjects plotted as a function of each subject’s αWM. Data from two experiments are shown: Gain −1.5 stimuli tested under background-absent conditions and Gain +1.5 stimuli tested under background-present conditions. The black line is the simulated DPM/DWM versus αWM, where the MSDPM=MSDWM but the random walk stimulus’s α is equal to one (Brownian motion) while the retina-contingent stimulus’s αWM ranged from 0.9 to 1.8. Both the model and data points show exponential decay.

References

    1. Legge Gordon E and Campbell FW. Displacement detection in human vision. Vision Research, 21(2):205–213, 1981. - PubMed
    1. Wang Yiyi, Bensaid Nicolas, Tiruveedhula Pavan, Ma Jianqiang, Ravikumar Sowmya, and Roorda Austin. Human foveal cone photoreceptor topography and its dependence on eye length. Elife, 8:e47148, 2019. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Westheimer Gerald. Visual acuity and hyperacuity. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 14(8):570–572, 1975. - PubMed
    1. McKee Suzanne P, Welch Leslie, Taylor Douglas G, and Bowne Samuel F. Finding the common bond: Stereoacuity and the other hyperacuities. Vision Research, 30(6):879–891, 1990. - PubMed
    1. Riggs Lorrin A, Ratliff Floyd, Cornsweet Janet C, and Cornsweet Tom N. The disappearance of steadily fixated visual test objects. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 43(6):495–501, 1953. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources