Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jul 8;14(1):15741.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-66438-7.

Development of full-body rhythmic synchronization in middle childhood

Affiliations

Development of full-body rhythmic synchronization in middle childhood

Jessica Phillips-Silver et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Rhythmic entrainment is a fundamental aspect of musical behavior, but the skills required to accurately synchronize movement to the beat seem to develop over many years. Motion capture studies of corporeal synchronization have shown immature abilities to lock in to the beat in children before age 5, and reliable synchronization ability in adults without musical training; yet there is a lack of data on full-body synchronization skills between early childhood and adulthood. To document typical rhythmic synchronization during middle childhood, we used a wireless motion capture device to measure period- and phase-locking of full body movement to rhythm and metronome stimuli in 6 to 11 year-old children in comparison with adult data. Results show a gradual improvement with age; however children's performance did not reach adult levels by age 12, suggesting that these skills continue to develop during adolescence. Our results suggest that in the absence of specific music training, full-body rhythmic entrainment skills improve gradually during middle childhood, and provide metrics for examining the continued maturation of these skills during adolescence.

Keywords: Child development; Motion capture; Rhythmic entrainment; Rhythmic synchronization.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Boxplots show results of analyses for period-locking (as measured by proportion of synchronized power) by age group for each stimulus: (a) slow metronome, (b) fast metronome, (c) slow rhythm, and (d) fast rhythm.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Boxplots show results of analyses for phase-locking (as measured by Rayleigh Z statistic) by age group for each stimulus: (a) slow metronome, (b) fast metronome, (c) slow rhythm, and (d) fast rhythm.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Results of the Von Mises distributions indicate the degree of peak phase shift, calculated for individual participants’ optimal beat levels, by age group for each stimulus: (a) slow metronome, (b) fast metronome, (c) slow rhythm, and (d) fast rhythm.

References

    1. Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S, editors. The origins of music. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2000.
    1. Clayton M, Sager R, Will U. In time with the music: The concept of entrainment and its significance for ethnomusicology. ESEM Counterpoint. 2004;1:1–84.
    1. Phillips-Silver J, Keller PE. Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Front. Human Neurosci. 2012 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00026. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Phillips-Silver J, Aktipis CA, Bryant GA. The ecology of entrainment: Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music. Percept. 2010;28:3–14. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.1.3. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Pressing J. Black atlantic rhythm: Its computational and transcultural foundations. Music. Percept. 2002;19:285–310. doi: 10.1525/mp.2002.19.3.285. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources