Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jan-Dec:28:23312165241262517.
doi: 10.1177/23312165241262517.

Is Recognition of Speech in Noise Related to Memory Disruption Caused by Irrelevant Sound?

Affiliations

Is Recognition of Speech in Noise Related to Memory Disruption Caused by Irrelevant Sound?

Daniel Oberfeld et al. Trends Hear. 2024 Jan-Dec.

Abstract

Listeners with normal audiometric thresholds show substantial variability in their ability to understand speech in noise (SiN). These individual differences have been reported to be associated with a range of auditory and cognitive abilities. The present study addresses the association between SiN processing and the individual susceptibility of short-term memory to auditory distraction (i.e., the irrelevant sound effect [ISE]). In a sample of 67 young adult participants with normal audiometric thresholds, we measured speech recognition performance in a spatial listening task with two interfering talkers (speech-in-speech identification), audiometric thresholds, binaural sensitivity to the temporal fine structure (interaural phase differences [IPD]), serial memory with and without interfering talkers, and self-reported noise sensitivity. Speech-in-speech processing was not significantly associated with the ISE. The most important predictors of high speech-in-speech recognition performance were a large short-term memory span, low IPD thresholds, bilaterally symmetrical audiometric thresholds, and low individual noise sensitivity. Surprisingly, the susceptibility of short-term memory to irrelevant sound accounted for a substantially smaller amount of variance in speech-in-speech processing than the nondisrupted short-term memory capacity. The data confirm the role of binaural sensitivity to the temporal fine structure, although its association to SiN recognition was weaker than in some previous studies. The inverse association between self-reported noise sensitivity and SiN processing deserves further investigation.

Keywords: binaural temporal fine structure sensitivity; irrelevant sound effect; noise sensitivity; speech perception in noise; variable importance measures; working memory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Data Availability StatementThe dataset used for the regression analyses is available at OSF.io (https://osf.io/daxvz/). The trial-by-trial data in the different experimental tasks are available from the corresponding author (DO), upon request. Declaration of Conflicting InterestsThe authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Distribution of individual better-ear pure tone average thresholds between 250 Hz and 4 kHz (PTABE, left panel), and the average bilateral asymmetry of the thresholds at the same octave frequencies (HLdiff, right panel).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Mean audiometric thresholds at octave frequencies between 125 and 4000 Hz. Squares: left ear. Triangles: right ear. Error bars show plus and minus one standard error of the mean (SEM) of the 67 individual values.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Distribution of individual speech-in-speech recognition scores (SRS; average proportion of correctly identified words in the OLSA matrix test) in the simulated cocktail-party listening task with two competing talkers.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Mean proportion correct in the serial recall task, as a function of item type (auditory or visual), Block (1 or 2), and sound condition (open squares: in quiet. Triangles: in the presence of two competing talkers). Error bars show ± 1 SEM.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Distribution of the irrelevant sound effect for auditory (ISEAud, upper panel) and visual (ISEVis, lower panel) item lists, expressed as the reduction in the proportion of correct responses in the serial recall task under irrelevant speech, relative to the in-quiet condition.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Distribution of short-term memory performance (PCSTM; average proportion of correctly recalled digits in the condition without interfering speech), averaged across auditorily and visually presented item lists.
Figure 7.
Figure 7.
Distribution of the thresholds for detecting interaural phase delays (IPDth).
Figure 8.
Figure 8.
Distribution of the noise sensitivity score (NSS; total item score on the questionnaire by Zimmer and Ellermeier, 1998).
Figure 9.
Figure 9.
Best subset selection. Histogram of the mean squared prediction error (MSPE) across the 256 subset models, averaged for each subset model across the 100 runs of four-fold cross validation. Upper panel: models not containing the predictor PCSTM. Lower panel: models containing PCSTM. Blue and orange histograms represent models not containing or containing the predictor NSS, respectively. The vertical blue line marks the fifth percentile. Models with an MSPE below this value are displayed in Table 2.
Figure 10.
Figure 10.
Comparison of dominance analysis and best subset selection results. Black circles: GDW computed for the full regression model (Table 1). Blue squares: pBSS from cross-validated best subset selection (Table 2). Same predictor names as in Table 1.

References

    1. Akeroyd M. (2008). Are individual differences in speech reception related to individual differences in cognitive ability? A survey of twenty experimental studies with normal and hearing-impaired adults. International Journal of Audiology, 47, S53–S71. 10.1080/14992020802301142 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Azen R., Budescu D. V. (2003). The dominance analysis approach for comparing predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Methods, 8(2), 129–148. 10.1037/1082-989X.8.2.129 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bartlett M. S. (1936). The square root transformation in analysis of variance. Supplement to the Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 3(1), 68–78.
    1. Beaman C. P., Jones D. M. (1997). Role of serial order in the irrelevant speech effect: Tests of the changing-state hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 23(2), 459–471. 10.1037/0278-7393.23.2.459 - DOI
    1. Békésy G. v. (1947). A new audiometer. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 35(5–6), 411–422. 10.3109/00016484709123756 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources