Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
[Preprint]. 2024 Aug 31:2024.07.30.605900.
doi: 10.1101/2024.07.30.605900.

Event boundaries drive norepinephrine release and distinctive neural representations of space in the rodent hippocampus

Affiliations

Event boundaries drive norepinephrine release and distinctive neural representations of space in the rodent hippocampus

Sam McKenzie et al. bioRxiv. .

Abstract

Episodic memories are temporally segmented around event boundaries that tend to coincide with moments of environmental change. During these times, the state of the brain should change rapidly, or reset, to ensure that the information encountered before and after an event boundary is encoded in different neuronal populations. Norepinephrine (NE) is thought to facilitate this network reorganization. However, it is unknown whether event boundaries drive NE release in the hippocampus and, if so, how NE release relates to changes in hippocampal firing patterns. The advent of the new GRABNE sensor now allows for the measurement of NE binding with sub-second resolution. Using this tool in mice, we tested whether NE is released into the dorsal hippocampus during event boundaries defined by unexpected transitions between spatial contexts and presentations of novel objections. We found that NE binding dynamics were well explained by the time elapsed after each of these environmental changes, and were not related to conditioned behaviors, exploratory bouts of movement, or reward. Familiarity with a spatial context accelerated the rate in which phasic NE binding decayed to baseline. Knowing when NE is elevated, we tested how hippocampal coding of space differs during these moments. Immediately after context transitions we observed relatively unique patterns of neural spiking which settled into a modal state at a similar rate in which NE returned to baseline. These results are consistent with a model wherein NE release drives hippocampal representations away from a steady-state attractor. We hypothesize that the distinctive neural codes observed after each event boundary may facilitate long-term memory and contribute to the neural basis for the primacy effect.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Interests The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Time from context transition controls SignalNE when mice are moved to novel arenas. A) Histological confirmation of GRABNE expression (GFP) and fiber placement over dorsal CA1. B) Schematic of experimental timeline. C) Example session showing increases in SignalNE around each context and homecage (HC) transition. D) Mean SignalNE measured across all transitions (black) and cross-validated prediction from the saturated model (red) or a reduced model lacking terms related to time from transfer (blue). E) Change in CVMSE due to removal of various potential behavioral variables. Only removal of the terms related to time from transition significantly decreased model performance (t(7) = 3.30, p = 0.01).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Time from context transition controls SignalNE when mice are moved to a linear track. A) Example session showing SignalNE (black) aligned with acceleration (red) and reward delivery (·). Vertical gray lines show that local peaks in SignalNE do not align to bouts of acceleration nor reward timing. Shaded area shows last 60s before removing from track during which SignalNE was not modeled. B) Mean SignalNE measured across all linear track transitions (black) and cross-validated prediction from the saturated model (red). C) Change in CVMSE due to removal of various potential behavioral variables. Only removal of the terms related to time from transition significantly decreased model performance (t(7) = 7.20,p = 0.0008).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Time from object introduction controls SignalNE A) Photographs of five novel objects presented to the mouse. B) Example session showing SignalNE (black) aligned object introduction (dashed line) and object sampling (·). C) Mean SignalNE measured across all object presentations (black) and cross-validated prediction from the saturated model (red). C) Change in CVMSE due to removal of various potential behavioral variables. Only removal of the terms related to time from object introduction significantly decreased model performance (t(5) = 3.54, p =0.017).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Novel objects do not affect NE dynamics after transfer to a familiar linear track. A) Mean SignalNE across experimental sessions when the track was baited with a novel object (black); control sessions were run without new objects (red). B) Estimated τ describing SignalNE decay after moving to the linear track did not change in the presence of a novel object (t(4) = 1.47, p = 0.22). C) Change in CVMSE due to removal of various potential behavioral variables. Only removal of the terms related to time from linear track transfer significantly decreased model performance (t(5) = 3.22, p = 0.03).
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Experience accelerates SignalNE decay after context transition. A) Mean SignalNE plotted as a function of time from context transition (dashed line) and color coded by number of days of experience. Black trace shows SignalNE recorded after transitioning back to the home cage (HC). B) Estimated SignalNE derived from the saturated model. C) Parameter estimates for the magnitude (β) and decay rate (τ) of SignalNE after context transition color-coded by days of experience. D) Decay rate (τ) after transfer to the arena hastens over days of exposure (mixed-effect linear model; t(73) = 2.31, p = 0.02) and is most rapid during transfer to the HC (Day N vs HC, all p ≤0.01).
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
SignalNE is depressed relative to baseline after periods of sustained elevation. A) Mean SignalNE recorded after moving mice back to the home cage from the arena (red) or the linear track (black). B) Same data as Figure 5C with the addition of parameter estimates for the behavior of SignalNE after transition to home cage from the linear track. C) The decrease in SignalNE was significantly larger after transitioning mice to the home cage from the linear track as compared to from the novel arenas (t(5) = 3.74, p = 0.005)
Figure 7.
Figure 7.
CA1 spatial code takes minutes to stabilize after context transition in novel and familiar spaces. A) Example UMAP embedding of population firing rate vectors (100-ms), color-coded by where the mouse was physically located on a linear track when the data was recorded. B) Same embedding color coded by time from context transfer. C) Representational similarity (Pearson R) of the observed population firing rate vector at each moment in a novel environment relative to the mean of the next 3 most similar vectors recorded in the same location. D) Same as Panel C recorded in a familiar environment. E) In a novel environment, the patterns recorded in the first minute were less correlated than those observed 10 minutes into the session (t(7) = 8.05, p = 0.00009) F) Same as Panel E recorded in a familiar environment (t(7) = 8.20, p = 0.00008). G) Initial representations were more correlated to their nearest neighbors in a familiar environment as compared to those recorded in a novel environment (t(7) = 7.58, p = 0.0001).
Figure 8.
Figure 8.
Moments immediately after transition are not preferentially replayed. A) Percentage of ripples recorded before (black) and after (red) experiencing a novel environment that showed significant reactivation of each moment after transition. Dashed line shows false positive (FP) rate. B) Moments recorded 10–11 minutes after novel context transition were more likely to be reactivated than those recorded 0–1 minutes after transition (t(7) = 2.46, p = 0.04). C) Same as Panel C showing reactivation rates as a function of time after transition to a familiar environment. D) There is no difference in reactivation rate for early vs late moment in a familiar environment (t(7) = 0.40, p = 0.70).

References

    1. Clewett D., Gasser C. & Davachi L. Pupil-linked arousal signals track the temporal organization of events in memory. Nat Commun 11, 4007, doi:10.1038/s41467-020-17851-9 (2020). - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hagena H., Hansen N. & Manahan-Vaughan D. beta-Adrenergic Control of Hippocampal Function: Subserving the Choreography of Synaptic Information Storage and Memory. Cereb Cortex 26, 1349–1364, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv330 (2016). - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Wang S. H., Redondo R. L. & Morris R. G. Relevance of synaptic tagging and capture to the persistence of long-term potentiation and everyday spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 19537–19542, doi:10.1073/pnas.1008638107 (2010). - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Lisman J. E. & Grace A. A. The hippocampal-VTA loop: controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron 46, 703–713, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.002 (2005). - DOI - PubMed
    1. Sajikumar S. & Frey J. U. Late-associativity, synaptic tagging, and the role of dopamine during LTP and LTD. Neurobiol Learn Mem 82, 12–25, doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2004.03.003 (2004). - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources