Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Aug;78(8):1501-1522.
doi: 10.1177/17470218241275977. Epub 2024 Aug 13.

Prosopagnosia is highly comorbid in individuals with probable developmental coordination disorder

Affiliations

Prosopagnosia is highly comorbid in individuals with probable developmental coordination disorder

Katherine Jane Maw et al. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2025 Aug.

Abstract

Developmental co-ordination disorder (DCD) is characterised by difficulties in motor control and coordination from early childhood. While problems processing facial identity are often associated with neurodevelopmental conditions, such issues have never been directly tested in adults with DCD. We tested this possibility through a range of tasks and assessed the prevalence of developmental prosopagnosia (i.e. lifelong difficulties with faces), in a group comprising individuals who self-reported a diagnosis of, or suspected that they had, DCD. Strikingly, we found 56% of this probable DCD group met recently recommended criteria for a diagnosis of prosopagnosia, with 22% acquiring a diagnosis using traditional cognitive task-based methods. Moreover, their problems with faces were apparent on both unfamiliar and familiar face memory tests, as well as on a facial perception task (i.e. could they tell faces apart). Positive correlations were found between self-report measures assessing movement and coordination problems, and objective difficulties on experimental face identity processing tasks, suggesting widespread neurocognitive disruption in DCD. Importantly, some issues in identity processing in our probable DCD group remained even after excluding participants with comorbid conditions traditionally associated with difficulties in face recognition, that is, autism and dyslexia. We recommend that any diagnostic test for DCD should include an assessment for prosopagnosia. Given the high prevalence of prosopagnosia in our probable DCD group, and the positive correlations between DCD and prosopagnosia symptoms, there may be a stronger link between movement and facial identity abilities than previously thought.

Keywords: DCD; developmental coordination disorder; dyspraxia; face perception; face recognition; motor; prosopagnosia.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of conflicting interestsThe author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Prosopagnosia Index results. Higher scores indicate more severe symptoms of prosopagnosia. PDCD participants reported significantly higher levels of symptoms than controls (p < .001). This was replicated in both the subgroups of pDCD participants without ASD and dyslexia (p = .003), and those with pDCD and comorbid dyslexia and autism (p < .001). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Probable DCD participants made significantly more errors on the upright Cambridge Face Perception Test (CFPT; p < .001), only after we added control data from Burns et al. (2023). This was replicated in a subgroup of those with pDCD and comorbid dyslexia and autism [p < .001], but not those without ASD and dyslexia (p = .09) although adding Burns et al. (2022) data did reveal problems in this subgroup (see text). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Probable DCD participants scores were significantly different than controls on the CFPT Holistic (p < .001) only after adding control data from Burns et al. (2023). This was replicated in the subgroup of pDCD with comorbid dyslexia or autism (p < .001) but not in pDCD participants without ASD and dyslexia (p = .07). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Probable DCD participants scored significantly fewer correct trials on the CFMT than controls (p = .002). This was replicated in the subgroup of pDCD participants with comorbid dyslexia and autism (p < .001), but not those without ASD and dyslexia (p = .14). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Probable DCD participants scored significantly lower on the FFT than controls (p < .001). Values are in percentages. This was replicated in the subgroup of those with dyslexia and autism (p < .001), but not pDCD participants without ASD and dyslexia (p = .12). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Top left panel indicates that increasing symptoms of DCD (AACQ) positively predicted prosopagnosia symptoms (PI20, r = .71, 95% CI = [0.57, 0.81], p < .001]. When analysed separately, the same correlation was not significant in the controls (white circles, r = −0.10, 95% CI = [−0.41, 0.23], p = .56), but was in DCD (blue circles, r = .51, 95% CI [0.19, 0.73], p = .003). Top right panel illustrates that increasing DCD symptomology (AACQ) negatively predicts ability to identify previously unknown faces (CFMT; r= −.33, 95% CI [−0.52, −0.1], p = .006). The lower left panel indicates that greater movement and coordination difficulty on the AACQ negatively predicted ability to recognise familiar faces on the FFT (r = −.34, 95% CI [−0.54, −0.12], p = .004).
Figure 7.
Figure 7.
The effect sizes of impairments in pDCD across all face and movement measures. The perception related problems with faces (e.g. CFPT Upright and Holistic) were comparable to the unfamiliar face memory problems (i.e. the CFMT). Then the pDCD group’s familiar face memory (FFT) problems were midway between these and their self-reported complaints with faces in daily life (i.e. PI20). Importantly, there were minimal differences between the pDCD and control groups, as shown by the very small CFPT inverted effect size. The two movement measures are plotted for comparison purposes.

Similar articles

References

    1. Abu-Akel A., Allison C., Baron-Cohen S., Heinke D. (2019). The distribution of autistic traits across the autism spectrum: Evidence for discontinuous dimensional subpopulations underlying the autism continuum. Molecular Autism, 10(1), 24. 10.1186/s13229-019-0275-3 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Adams A., Hills P. J., Bennetts R. J., Bate S. (2020). Coping strategies for developmental prosopagnosia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 30(10), 1996–2015. 10.1080/09602011.2019.1623824 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Åsberg Johnels J., Hadjikhani N., Sundqvist M., Galazka M. A. (2022). Face processing in school children with dyslexia: Neuropsychological and eye-tracking findings. Developmental Neuropsychology, 47(2), 78–92. - PubMed
    1. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Publishing.
    1. Astle D. E., Fletcher-Watson S. (2020). Beyond the core-deficit hypothesis in developmental disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(5), 431–437. 10.1177/0963721420925518 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources