The influence of cardiac synchronisation on self-attribution to external objects in male participants
- PMID: 39282685
- PMCID: PMC11394186
- DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1442942
The influence of cardiac synchronisation on self-attribution to external objects in male participants
Abstract
Interoception, the representation of our bodily state derived from physiological signals, is fundamental to our sense of self. Previous studies using cardiac feedback paradigms demonstrated interoceptive effects on self-perception. However, it remains unclear whether interoceptive information can extend self-attribution to non-bodily objects. This study aimed to elucidate whether cardiac signals can induce self-attribution to non-bodily objects and how interoceptive accuracy modulates this effect. A total of 44 male volunteers participated in an emotion assignment task where they viewed images of palms (bodily targets) and spheres (non-bodily targets) flashing in or out of sync with their heartbeat and assigned emotional images (positive/negative) to these targets. A heartbeat discrimination task was used to measure the interoceptive accuracy. The results showed no significant effect of synchronisation on emotion assignment for either the target type or the valence of the emotional images. However, participants with high interoceptive accuracy attributed both positive and negative images more to synchronised targets than those with low interoceptive accuracy. These findings suggest that although cardiac synchronisation may not uniformly facilitate the self-attribution of external objects, interoceptive accuracy may mediate attention to synchrony. Future studies should explore the conditions under which cardiac signals influence self-attribution.
Keywords: approach-avoidance; cardiac feedback; interoception; interoceptive accuracy; self-attribution; sense of self.
Copyright © 2024 Shibata, Harada and Ohira.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Figures




Similar articles
-
Interoceptive influences on peripersonal space boundary.Cognition. 2018 Aug;177:79-86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.001. Epub 2018 Apr 11. Cognition. 2018. PMID: 29655026
-
Heartfelt embodiment: Changes in body-ownership and self-identification produce distinct changes in interoceptive accuracy.Cognition. 2017 Feb;159:1-10. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.11.002. Epub 2016 Nov 20. Cognition. 2017. PMID: 27880880
-
Interoceptive Awareness Is Negatively Related to the Exteroceptive Manipulation of Bodily Self-Location.Front Psychol. 2020 Dec 4;11:562016. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.562016. eCollection 2020. Front Psychol. 2020. PMID: 33343444 Free PMC article.
-
Differentiating attention styles and regulatory aspects of self-reported interoceptive sensibility.Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016 Nov 19;371(1708):20160013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0013. Epub 2016 Oct 10. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016. PMID: 28080970 Free PMC article. Review.
-
How might interoceptive accuracy training work?Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2025 May;172:106126. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2025.106126. Epub 2025 Mar 25. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2025. PMID: 40147694 Review.
References
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources