Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Aug 27;14(9):754.
doi: 10.3390/bs14090754.

Young Children's Directed Question Asking in Preschool Classrooms

Affiliations

Young Children's Directed Question Asking in Preschool Classrooms

Michelle Wong et al. Behav Sci (Basel). .

Abstract

Question asking is a prevalent aspect of children's speech, providing a means by which young learners can rapidly gain information about the world. Previous research has demonstrated that children exhibit sensitivity to the knowledge state of potential informants in laboratory settings. However, it remains unclear whether and how young children are inclined to direct questions that support learning deeper content to more knowledgeable informants in naturalistic classroom contexts. In this study, we examined children's question-asking targets (adults, other preschoolers, self-talk) during an open-play period in a US preschool classroom and assessed how the cognitive and linguistic characteristics of questions varied as a function of the intended recipient. Further, we examined how these patterns changed with age. We recorded the spontaneous speech of individual children between the ages of 3 and 6 years (N = 30, totaling 2875 utterances) in 40-min open-period sessions in their preschool day, noting whether the speech was directed toward an adult, another child, or was stated to self. We publish this fully transcribed database with contextual and linguistic details coded as open access to all future researchers. We found that questions accounted for a greater proportion of preschoolers' adult-directed speech than of their child-directed and self-directed speech, with a particular increase in questions that supported broader learning goals when directed to an adult. Younger children directed a higher proportion of learning questions to adults than themselves, whereas older children asked similar proportions of questions to both, suggesting a difference in younger and older children's question-asking strategies. Although children used greater lexical diversity in questions than in other utterances, their question formulation in terms of length and diversity remained consistent across age and recipient types, reflecting their general linguistic abilities. Our findings reveal that children discriminately choose "what" and "whom" to ask in daily spontaneous conversations. Even in less-structured school contexts, preschoolers direct questions to the informant most likely to be able to provide an adequate answer.

Keywords: classroom observation; preschoolers; question asking; social environments; spontaneous speech.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Relative frequency of utterance length in words for questions (on the right) and non-questions (on the left).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Relative frequency of the length of utterances in words for communication (left), information (middle), and learning (right) questions.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Proportion of children’s utterances including questions as a function of recipient type. The proportion of questions over other possible speech acts based on the recipient of speech production were presented to control for the difference in the amount of speech directed to recipients and uneven child-to-adult ratio in each classroom. That is, the total number of speech acts a child generated for each recipient type was computed first, and then the subset of these acts that were questions were taken as a proportion of that total. Error bars denote SE.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Proportion of questions intended for communication (left), information (middle), and learning (right) as a function of recipient. The proportion of questions relative to other speech acts based on the recipient of the speech was presented to account for the differences in the amount of speech directed to various recipients and the uneven child-to-adult ratio in each classroom. Error bars denote SE.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Predicted probability of utterances including questions as a function of age and recipient type. Shaded areas denote 95% CI.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Predicted probability of utterances including learning questions as a function of age and recipient type. Shaded areas denote 95% CI.

Similar articles

References

    1. Callanan M., Solis G., Castañeda C., Jipson J. Children’s Question-Asking across Cultural Communities. In: Butler L.P., Ronfard S., Corriveau K.H., editors. The Questioning Child: Insights from Psychology and Education. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2020. pp. 73–88.
    1. Hood L., Bloom L., Brainerd C.J. What, when, and how about why: A longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1979;44:1–47. doi: 10.2307/1165989. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Chouinard M.M., Harris P.L., Maratsos M.P. Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2007;72:i-129. - PubMed
    1. Piaget J. Psychology. Monist. 1926;36:430–455. doi: 10.5840/monist192636333. - DOI
    1. Ronfard S., Zambrana I.M., Hermansen T.K., Kelemen D. Question-asking in childhood: A review of the literature and a framework for understanding its development. Dev. Rev. 2018;49:101–120. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.002. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources