Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Nov;86(8):2707-2720.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-024-02956-w. Epub 2024 Sep 30.

The influence of "advancing" and "receding" colors on figure-ground perception under monocular and binocular viewing

Affiliations

The influence of "advancing" and "receding" colors on figure-ground perception under monocular and binocular viewing

Jaeseon Song et al. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2024 Nov.

Abstract

Research on figure-ground perception has consistently found that red images are more likely to be perceived as figure/nearer, yet the mechanisms behind this are not completely clear. The primary theories have pointed to optical chromatic aberrations or cortical mechanisms, such as the antagonistic interactions of the magno-/parvocellular (M/P) systems. Our study explored this color-biased figure-ground perception by examining the duration for which a region was perceived as figure under both binocular and monocular conditions, using all combinations of red, blue, green, and gray. In Experiment 1, we used figure-ground ambiguous Maltese crosses, composed of left- and right-tilting sectors of equal area. In Experiment 2, the crosses were figure-ground biased with size and orientation cues. Here, small sectors of cardinal orientations, likely perceived as figure, were contrasted with larger, obliquely oriented sectors, likely perceived as ground. Under monocular conditions, the results aligned with chromatic aberration predictions: red advanced and blue receded, regardless of size and orientation. However, under binocular conditions, the advancing effect of red continued, but the receding effect of blue was generally not observed. Notably, blue, along with red and green, was more frequently perceived as figure compared to gray. The results under binocular viewing are in line with the expectations of the antagonistic M/P system interactions theory, likely due to the collective input from both eyes, facilitating the anticipated effects. Our findings suggest that color-biased figure-ground perception may arise from the synergistic effect of antagonistic M/P system interactions and other optical and cortical mechanisms, together compensating for chromatic aberrations.

Keywords: Chromatic aberration; Figure-ground perception; Magnocellular; Parvocellular.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Ethics approval: Approval was obtained from the Institutional Review Board at the University of Georgia. The procedures used in this study adhere to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Consent to participate: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Consent to publication: Not applicable. Conflicts of interest: The authors have no relevant financial or nonfinancial interests to disclose.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Examples of two types of patterns for the red and gray (Rd/Gy) pair used in our experiments. (a) An example of the ambiguous Maltese cross pattern from Experiment 1. This pattern comprises left- and right-tilting crosses of equal size, creating ambiguity in figure-ground perception. (b) An example of the Maltese cross pattern with figure-ground bias from Experiment 2, integrating size and orientation cues, featuring a smaller, cardinal oriented "plus-shaped cross," indicative of figures, and a larger, obliquely oriented "X-shaped cross," suggestive of grounds
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Durations the alternate crosses were perceived as figure in Experiment 1. Durations the alternate crosses were perceived as figure (a) under monocular viewing and (b) under binocular viewing, where the two crosses were figure-ground ambiguous, consisting of left- and right-tilting sectors of equal area. Error bars = ± SEM
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Durations the alternate crosses were perceived as figure in Experiment 2. Durations the alternate crosses were perceived as figure (a) under monocular viewing and (b) under binocular viewing, where the two crosses were figure-ground biased. In each figure-ground color combination, the bars on the left show the durations for Plus-shaped crosses, biased to be seen as figure, and the bars on the right show the X-shaped crosses, biased to be seen as ground. Error bars = ± SEM

References

    1. Anderson, S. F., Kelley, K., & Maxwell, S. E. (2017). Sample-size planning for more accurate statistical power: A method adjusting sample effect sizes for publication bias and uncertainty. Psychological Science,28(11), 1547–1562. 10.1177/0956797617723724 - PubMed
    1. Artal, P., Chen, L., Fernández, E. J., Singer, B., Manzanera, S., & Williams, D. R. (2004). Neural compensation for the eye’s optical aberrations. Journal of Vision,4(4), 281–287. 10.1167/4.4.4 - PubMed
    1. Bedwell, J. S., Brown, J. M., & Orem, D. M. (2008). The effect of a red background on location backward masking by structure. Perception & Psychophysics,70(3), 503–507. 10.3758/pp.70.3.503 - PubMed
    1. Benedi-Garcia, C., Vinas, M., Dorronsoro, C., Burns, S. A., Peli, E., & Marcos, S. (2021). Vision is protected against blue defocus. Scientific Reports,11(1), 1–9. 10.1038/s41598-020-79911-w - PMC - PubMed
    1. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological),57(1), 289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

LinkOut - more resources