Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Oct 9;11(10):240662.
doi: 10.1098/rsos.240662. eCollection 2024 Oct.

Comparing self-other distinction across motor, cognitive and affective domains

Affiliations

Comparing self-other distinction across motor, cognitive and affective domains

Ekaterina Pronizius et al. R Soc Open Sci. .

Abstract

The self-other distinction (SOD) is a process by which humans disentangle self from other-related mental representations. This online study investigated two unresolved questions: (i) whether partially the same processes underpin SOD for motor, cognitive and affective representations, and (ii) whether SOD overlaps with domain-general cognitive control processes. Participants (N = 243) performed three SOD tasks (motor: automatic imitation inhibition (AIT); cognitive: visual perspective-taking (VPT); affective: emotional egocentricity bias (av-EEB) tasks) and two cognitive control tasks (Stroop and stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) tasks). Correlation analyses showed no associations among the motor, cognitive and affective SOD indexes. Similarly, distinct SOD clusters emerged in the hierarchical clustering dendrogram, indicating clear separations among SODs. However, the results of multidimensional scaling suggested a tendency towards two clusters, as evidenced by the proximity of AIT and VPT indexes in relation to EEB indexes. AIT spatial laterality and Stroop domain-general cognitive control confounded AIT and VPT indexes, albeit slightly differently depending on the analysis method used. SSRT showed neither associations with SODs nor with other domain-general indexes. These findings underscore the complexity of SOD processes and have notable implications for basic and applied research, e.g. in the domain of clinical disorders affected by deficiencies in SOD.

Keywords: automatic imitation; cognitive control; emotional egocentricity bias; self–other distinction; visual perspective-taking.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

We declare we have no competing interests.

Figures

Note. A trial starts with an intertrial interval, followed by a frame depicting a static hand alongside a white fixation square.
Figure 1.
Timeline of (a) an Experimental Trial; (b) a Base Trial. Note: A trial starts with an intertrial interval, followed by a frame depicting a static hand alongside a white fixation square. In the experimental trials (a), the next frame shows a lifted finger. In the base trials (b), the stimulus hand remained static and was additionally pixelated. In this case, an orange fixation square provides a cue for a finger response (group 1: middle finger, group 2: index finger lift). The trial ends with a 1520 ms inter-stimulus interval. The next trial starts only when both N and M keys are pressed. Adapted from [, fig. 1]; Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License.
Note. The trial starts with a fixation cross, followed by an inter-stimulus interval (ISI).
Figure 2.
Timeline of an experimental congruent trial. Note: The trial starts with a fixation cross, followed by an inter-stimulus interval (ISI). The next frame indicates to the participants that they have to adopt another person’s perspective (she) and memorize the digit 2. After the second ISI, the participant sees a gender-matched avatar (e.g. a woman) facing the wall with the two red discs on it. The correct response is yes since the memorized digit 2 matches the number of discs the avatar can see. In this example, the participant and the avatar see the same number of discs (congruent condition). Adapted from [46], stimuli: Samson & Apperly, 2015, Figshare [59].
Note. A trial starts with an intertrial interval, followed by a picture depicting an applauding crowd.
Figure 3.
Timeline of an experimental incongruent self-trial. Note. A trial starts with an intertrial interval, followed by a picture depicting an applauding crowd. The text on the top of the picture indicates that the paired fake participant MAPR82 is listening to sounds associated with this picture (in this case, of a positive valence). The real participant is listening to the sound of a woman being attacked (negative valence), making it an incongruent trial. After 3000 ms, the participant had to rate their feelings induced by the attack audio stimulation (self-rating). Based on the study by [47], adapted from https://psyarxiv.com/j7vec/, CC0 1.0 Universal.
Note. The SSRT variable is not displayed on this graph due to its significant distance from other variables.
Figure 4.
Results of the hierarchical cluster analysis. Note: The SSRT variable is not displayed on this graph due to its significant distance from other variables (53.99). Its exclusion is intentional to enhance graph clarity. In the provided dendrogram, a horizontal line is drawn at a height of 0.35. This cut-off was selected based on the largest vertical gaps between merges, indicating significant separations between clusters. By examining the dendrogram, it is evident that cutting at this height allows for the identification of distinct groups within the data.
Note. The SSRT score is not displayed on this graph due to its significant distance from other scores.
Figure 5.
Results of the multidimensional scaling. Note: The SSRT score is not displayed on this graph due to its significant distance from other scores (Dim1: 49.49, Dim2: 0.004). Its exclusion is intentional to enhance graph clarity. hc_clusters = Clusters identified through hierarchical clustering analysis.

Similar articles

Cited by

  • Basis functions for complex social decisions in dorsomedial frontal cortex.
    Wittmann MK, Lin Y, Pan D, Braun MN, Dickson C, Spiering L, Luo S, Harbison C, Abdurahman A, Hamilton S, Faber NS, Khalighinejad N, Lockwood PL, Rushworth MFS. Wittmann MK, et al. Nature. 2025 May;641(8063):707-717. doi: 10.1038/s41586-025-08705-9. Epub 2025 Mar 12. Nature. 2025. PMID: 40074892 Free PMC article.

References

    1. Frith CD. 2008. Social cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 2033–2039. (10.1098/rstb.2008.0005) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Lamm C, Bukowski H, Silani G. 2016. From shared to distinct self-other representations in empathy: evidence from neurotypical function and socio-cognitive disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150083. (10.1098/rstb.2015.0083) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Steinbeis N. 2016. The role of self-other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150074. (10.1098/rstb.2015.0074) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Eddy CM. 2022. The transdiagnostic relevance of self-other distinction to psychiatry spans emotional, cognitive and motor domains. Front. Psychiatry 13, 797952. (10.3389/fpsyt.2022.797952) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T. 2011. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502. (10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014) - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources