Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Mar;66(3):333-349.
doi: 10.1111/jcpp.14062. Epub 2024 Nov 4.

Callous-unemotional traits, cognitive functioning, and externalizing problems in a propensity-matched sample from the ABCD study

Affiliations

Callous-unemotional traits, cognitive functioning, and externalizing problems in a propensity-matched sample from the ABCD study

Kristin Murtha et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2025 Mar.

Abstract

Background: Many studies show that both callous-unemotional (CU) traits (e.g., low empathy, lack of guilt) and cognitive difficulties increase risk for externalizing psychopathology across development. However, other work suggests that some aggression (e.g., relational, proactive) may rely on intact cognitive function, which could vary based on the presence of CU traits. Moreover, no prior research has adequately accounted for common risk factors shared by CU traits, cognitive difficulties, and externalizing problems, which confounds conclusions that can be drawn about their purported relationships. The current study addressed these knowledge gaps by leveraging rigorous propensity matching methods to isolate associations between CU traits and different dimensions of cognitive function and externalizing problems.

Methods: Associations between CU traits, cognitive functioning, and externalizing outcomes were tested within dimensional (n = 11,868) and propensity-matched group-based (n = 1,224) models using data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study®, with rigorous statistical control for shared sociodemographic risk factors. Cross-sectional outcomes were parent-reported symptoms of conduct disorder (CD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Longitudinal outcomes were child-reported overt and relational aggression.

Results: CU traits were uniquely related to more parent-reported CD, ODD, ADHD symptoms, as well as more child-reported aggressive behaviors. Effects of cognitive difficulties were domain specific and were not consistent across dimensional and propensity matched models. There was minimal evidence for divergent associations between CU traits and externalizing outcomes as a function of cognition (i.e., no moderation).

Conclusions: Rigorous control for sociodemographic factors within propensity-matched models establish CU traits as a robust and unique risk factor for externalizing psychopathology, over and above difficulties with cognitive functioning.

Keywords: aggressive behavior; callous‐unemotional traits; cognitive function; externalizing disorders.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Main effects of CU traits, general ability, executive function, and learning and memory on externalizing and aggressive behavior in dimensional models. (A) Dimensional CU traits were related to higher levels of ADHD (β = .29, p < .001), CD (β = .40, p < .001) and ODD (β = .42, p < .001) symptoms. Lower general ability was related to more ADHD (β = −.10, p < .001) and CD symptoms (β = −.08, p < .001). Lower executive function was related to more ADHD (β = −.09, p < .001), CD (β = −.03, p < .01), and ODD symptoms (β = −.03, p < .01). Lower learning and memory were also related to more ADHD (β = −.08, p < .001), CD (β = −.04, p < .001), and ODD symptoms (β = −.03, p < .01). (B) Dimensional CU traits were related to more child‐reported overt (β = .09, p < .001) and relational (β = .06, p < .001) aggression. Higher general ability was related to higher levels of relational aggression (β = .09, p < .001). Lower executive function was related to higher overt aggression (β = −.04, p < .001). Higher learning and memory was related to higher relational aggression (β = .02, p < .05) and lower overt aggression (β = .−02, p < .05)
Figure 2
Figure 2
Interaction between CU traits and general cognition predicts CD symptoms. Simple slopes plotted at mean levels (no CU traits), 1 SD above the mean, and 2 SD above the mean, calculated with an online computational tool (Preacher et al., 2006). Lower levels of general cognition were more strongly related to CD symptoms for children with the highest levels of CU traits (B = −0.42, SE = .13, p < .01), than for children with lower levels (B = −0.31, SE = .08, p < .001) or no CU traits (B = −0.20, SE = .03, p < .001)
Figure 3
Figure 3
Propensity score matching procedures generate CU+ and CU− groups that do not differ significantly on any sociodemographic or methodological variables. (A) Matching procedures meant that absolute standardized mean differences between groups were rendered <.10. (B) Matching on covariates improved similarities between distributions of propensity scores between matched and unmatched groups
Figure 4
Figure 4
Main effects of CU traits group, general ability, executive function, and learning and memory on externalizing and aggressive behavior in propensity matched models. (A) Propensity Matched CU traits group was related to higher levels of ADHD (β = .40, p < .001), CD (β = .46, p < .001), and ODD symptoms (β = .55, p < .001). Lower general ability was related to higher levels of ADHD (β = −.10, p < .01) and CD symptoms (β = −.09, p < .01). Lower executive function was related to higher levels of ADHD symptoms (β = −.08, p < .05). Lower learning and memory was related to lower levels of ADHD (β = −.09, p < .01), CD (β = −.06, p < .05), and ODD symptoms (β = −.08, p < .01). (B) Propensity Matched CU traits group was related to more (β = .16, p < .001) and relational (β = .13, p < .05) aggression
Figure 5
Figure 5
Effect sizes for the association between CU traits and externalizing problems were significantly larger in size for the propensity‐matched group‐based versus dimensional models for child‐reported overt aggression and parent‐reported CD, ODD, and ADHD symptoms. Significant difference was determined by the overlap of independent confidence intervals (Cumming, 2009)

Similar articles

References

    1. Achenbach, T.M. , & Ruffle, T.M. (2000). The Child Behavior Checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatrics in Review, 21, 265–271. - PubMed
    1. Austin, G. , Bondü, R. , & Elsner, B. (2020). Executive function, theory of mind, and conduct‐problem symptoms in middle childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 539. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Austin, P.C. (2011). An introduction to propensity score methods for reducing the effects of confounding in observational studies. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46, 399–424. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barch, D.M. , Albaugh, M.D. , Avenevoli, S. , Chang, L. , Clark, D.B. , Glantz, M.D. , … & Sher, K.J. (2018). Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 55–66. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Baskin‐Sommers, A.R. , Waller, R. , Fish, A.M. , & Hyde, L.W. (2015). Callous‐unemotional traits trajectories interact with earlier conduct problems and executive control to predict violence and substance use among high risk male adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1529–1541. - PubMed

MeSH terms

LinkOut - more resources