Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Nov 14;19(11):e0310835.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0310835. eCollection 2024.

Misperception of the facial appearance that the opposite-sex desires

Affiliations

Misperception of the facial appearance that the opposite-sex desires

David I Perrett et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Thin and muscular have been characterised as body shape ideals for women and men, respectively, yet each sex misperceives what the other sex desires; women exaggerate the thinness that men like and men exaggerate the muscularity that women like. Body shape ideals align with stereotypic perceptions of femininity in women and masculinity in men. The present study investigates whether misperception of opposite-sex desires extends to femininity/masculinity in facial morphology. We used interactive 3D head models to represent faces varying in sexual dimorphism. White European heterosexual men and women were asked to choose their own and ideal face shape, the ideal shape of a short- and a long-term partner, and the face shape they thought the opposite sex would most like for a short- and a long-term partner. Women overestimated the facial femininity that men prefer in a partner and men overestimated the facial masculinity that women prefer in a partner. The discrepancy between own and ideal sexual dimorphism (an index of appearance dissatisfaction) covaried with by the misperception of what the opposite-sex desires. These results indicate misperception of opposite-sex facial preferences and that mistaken perceptions may contribute to dissatisfaction with own appearance.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Transformation of sexually dimorphic face shape.
Upper: The average male face transformed by −100% (feminised), 0% (neutral), +100% (masculinised) and +200% (masculinised) sexual dimorphism. Lower: The average female face transformed by −100% (masculinised), 0% (neutral), +100% (feminised) and +200% (feminised) sexual dimorphism. To preserve anonymity of participants, these images are averages of the shape, colour and texture of 50 male faces and 68 female faces, respectively, rather than of real individuals.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Male facial masculinity.
Left: Men’s predictions of women’s preferences for short- and long-term relationships. Right: Women’s masculinity preference for short- and long-term relationships. Histogram bars give the mean and SE of the percentage masculinity chosen, with 0% corresponding to the original base face.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Male facial masculinity chosen.
Men’s prediction of women’s preferences (left) and women’s masculinity preference (right) for short-term and long-term relationship contexts. Numbers in white display the mean amount of sexual dimorphism transform applied. The facial images are averages of the shape, colour and texture of 50 male faces and 68 female faces, respectively, rather than of real individuals.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Female facial femininity.
Left: women’s predictions of men’s preferences for short- and long-term relationships. Right: Men’s masculinity preference for short- and long-term relationships. Histogram bars give the mean and SE percentage masculinity chosen, with 0% corresponding to the original base face.
Fig 5
Fig 5. Female facial femininity chosen.
(Left) Women’s prediction of men’s preferences for short-term and long-term relationship contexts. (Right) Men’s femininity preference for short-term and long-term relationship contexts. The facial images are averages of the shape, colour and texture of 50 male faces and 68 female faces, respectively, rather than of real individuals.

References

    1. Swami V. Cultural influences on body size ideals. Eur Psychol. 2015;20(1):44–51. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000150 - DOI
    1. Thompson JK, Cafri G, editors. The muscular ideal: Psychological, social, and medical perspectives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2007. doi: 10.1037/11581-000 - DOI
    1. Wharton CM, Adams T, Hampl JS. Weight loss practices and body weight perceptions among US college students. J Am Coll Health. 2008;56:579–84. doi: 10.3200/JACH.56.5.579-584 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Cafri G, van den Berg P, Thompson JK. Pursuit of muscularity in adolescent boys: Relations among biopsychosocial variables and clinical outcomes. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2006;35(2):283–91. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_12 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Kanayama G, Hudson JI, Pope HG Jr. Anabolic-androgenic steroid use and body image in men: A growing concern for clinicians. Psychother Psychosom. 2020;89(2):65–73. doi: 10.1159/000505978 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources