Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Dec 18;24(1):809.
doi: 10.1186/s12887-024-05303-y.

Association of anthropometric parameters with intelligence quotient in early school-aged children: a cross-sectional study in Iran

Affiliations

Association of anthropometric parameters with intelligence quotient in early school-aged children: a cross-sectional study in Iran

Kimia Mahjubi et al. BMC Pediatr. .

Abstract

Background: While the link between body growth indices in the first three years of life and neurodevelopment is well established, the relationship between these indicators and intelligence levels in later stages is not well understood. This study aimed to investigate the association between various anthropometric indices and Intelligence Quotient in early school-aged children.

Methods: This cross-sectional study recruited healthy students aged 7 to 8 years from four primary schools in Tehran, Iran. Measurements included body weight, height, body mass index, arm circumference, waist circumference, and hip circumference. Intelligence Quotient was assessed using the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices Test, with scores standardized by age according to the Raven's manual. Separate univariate linear regression analyses evaluated the association between each anthropometric index and children's Intelligence Quotient scores. Multivariate linear regression analyses were then performed to adjust for potential confounding factors, including the child's sex, age, and parental education levels.

Results: A cohort of 160 children (80 females), with a mean age of 7.5 ± 0.6 years, was included in the study. The participants exhibited the following mean anthropometric values: weight 27.7 ± 6.5 kg, height 1.2 ± 0.1 m, body mass index 17.9 ± 3.4 kg/m2, arm circumference 20.7 ± 2.8 cm, waist circumference 56.4 ± 6.4 cm, and hip circumference 68.1 ± 6.6 cm. Their mean Intelligence Quotient score was 106 ± 16.6. In the univariate analysis, children's weight was not significantly associated with Intelligence Quotient score (P-value = 0.153, β = 0.288 [-0.108, 0.684]), while height was negatively associated (P-value = 0.048, β = -31.685 [-63.142, -0.228]) and body mass index showed a positive association (P-value = 0.001, β = 1.265 [0.524, 2.006]). Abdominal circumference was not significantly associated with Intelligence Quotient (P-value = 0.913, β = 0.051 [-0.870, 0.972]), but both waist circumference (P-value = 0.007, β = 0.542 [0.147, 0.937]) and hip circumference (P-value = 0.013, β = 0.484 [0.102, 0.866]) demonstrated significant positive associations with Intelligence Quotient. However, none of the anthropometric indices maintained statistically significant associations with Intelligence Quotient after adjusting for potential confounders (P-values > 0.05).

Conclusions: This study indicated no strong relationship between various body measurements and intelligence levels in school-aged children after adjusting the analyses for confounders. This suggests that intelligence in these children may be largely shaped by genetic and sociodemographic factors, with minimal influence from physical growth patterns.

Keywords: Body mass index; Body weight; Childhood obesity; Intelligence quotient; Raven's progressive matrices.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Ethics approval and consent to participate: All stages of this study were performed in studies involving human participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Prior to enrollment, the study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) and ethics committee of the Tehran University of Medical Sciences with the reference number IR.TUMS.1394.1016. Prior to participation, verbal and written informed consent from parents and verbal agreement from students were obtained, following a detailed presentation of the study's purpose and comprehensive explanations regarding the information sought in the questionnaire. Consent for publication: After explaining the purpose of the study, verbal and written informed consent was obtained from the parents of the students for the anonymous publication of their children’s data. Competing interests: The authors declare no competing interests.

Similar articles

References

    1. Harris MA, Brett CE, Deary IJ, Starr JM. Associations among height, body mass index and intelligence from age 11 to age 78 years. BMC Geriatr. 2016;16(1):167. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Jacob L, Haro JM, Smith L, Koyanagi A. Association between intelligence quotient and obesity in England. Lifestyle Medicine. 2020;1(2):e11.
    1. Edlow AG. Maternal obesity and neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders in offspring. Prenat Diagn. 2017;37(1):95–110. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cusick SE, Georgieff MK. The role of nutrition in brain development: the golden opportunity of the “First 1000 Days.” J Pediatr. 2016;175:16–21. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Widen EM, Nichols AR, Kahn LG, et al. Prepregnancy obesity is associated with cognitive outcomes in boys in a low-income, multiethnic birth cohort. BMC Pediatr. 2019;19(1):507. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources