Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Dec 28;14(1):31003.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-82108-0.

Assessment of retention and attenuation of motor-learning memory by repeated rotor-rod analyses

Affiliations

Assessment of retention and attenuation of motor-learning memory by repeated rotor-rod analyses

Sho Kakizawa. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Retention of acquired learning memory is essential for reasonable behavior and crisis avoidance of individuals. Therefore, establishment of a system suitable for analysis of the retention/attenuation of acquired memory is desired. In the present study, mice were conducted on the repeated rotor-rod test, consisting of two series of experiments (Series 1 and 2) of 10 trials each. When rotating speed was 9 rpm, retention time on the rod was gradually increased and reached the maximum value within 10 trials in Series 1. When Series 2 was performed 1 or 3 days after Series 1, retention time of trials 1-3 in Series 2 was not significantly different from that of trials 8-10 in Series 1. On the other hand, retention time of trials 1-3 in Series 2 was significantly declined from that of trials 8-10 in Series 1 when Series 2 was conducted day 7, and returned to the initial level, the same level with trials 1-3 in Series 1, when Series 2 was conducted on days 14 or 30. These results indicate that acquired motor-learning memory is retained for 3 days at least, began to decline by day 7 and returned to the initial level by day 14. In older mice of 10-11 months old, there was a delay in the acquisition of motor learning in Day 0, whereas the retention was not impaired in Day 7. The repeated rotor-rod analyses may useful for research on factors affecting retention/attenuation and acquisition of motor-learning memory and proceed our understanding of motor-learning memory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Competing interests: The authors declare no competing financial and non-financial interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Effect of speed of rotating rod on retention time on the rod. (AE) Retention time of the mice placed on the rod rotating at 7 (A), 8 (B), 9 (C), 10 (D) and 11 (E) rpm. Mean ± S.E.M. (n = 10 in each group). Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test was used for statistical analysis, after one-way ANOVA.
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Retention time of mice conducted on 2 series of rotor-rod tests. Series of rotor-rod test (9 rpm) consisting of 10 trials were repeated two times with interval of 1 (A; n = 10), 3 (B; n = 12), 7 (C; n = 12), 14 (D; n = 10) or 30 (E; n = 8) days. Retention time of each trial in the first series (Day 0; left column) and second series (Day 1, 3, 7, 14 or 30; right column) was expressed as Mean ± S.E.M.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Retention/attenuation of motor-learning memory on days 1–30. (AE) Averaged retention times of trials 1–3, 4–7 and 8–10 in series 1 (left column; Day 0) and series 2 (right column; Day 1 (A; n = 10), 3 (B; n = 12), 7 (C; n = 12), 14 (D; n = 10) or 30 (E; n = 8)). Mean ± S.E.M. (n = 7 in each group). ++p < 0.01, +++p < 0.001, compared with the value of trials 1–3 in Series 1, and *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01,***p < 0.001, compared with the value of trials 8–10 in Series 1 by Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test after one-way repeated measures ANOVA.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Decline in motor-learning memory from Day 1 to Day 30. Retention times of trials 1–3 (A), 4–7 (B), 8–10 (C) in Series 2, normalized by value of trials 8–10 in Series 1. Rotor-rod test in Series 2 were conducted 1 (n = 10), 3 (n = 8), 7 (n = 9), 14 (n = 8) or 30 (n = 8) days after the Series 1. Mean ± S.E.M. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, compared with the value of Day 1 by Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test after one-way ANOVA.
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Effects of aging on motor learning analyzed by the repeated rotor-rod test. Young (Y; n = 12) and aged (A; n = 8) mice were conducted on 2 series of rotor-rod tests (9 rpm) with interval of 7 days. (A, B) Retention time of each trial in the first series (left column; Day 0; young (Y) and aged (A) mice) and second series (right column; Day 7) was expressed as Mean ± S.E.M. Data of young mice were the same with those in Fig. 2C. (C, D) Averaged retention times of trials 1–3, 4–7 and 8–10 in series 1 (left column; Day 0) and series 2 (right column; Day 7) of young (Y) and aged (A) mice. Mean ± S.E.M. Data of young mice were the same with those in Fig. 3C. ++p < 0.01, +++p < 0.001, compared with the value of trials 1–3 in Series 1, and *p < 0.05, compared with the value of trials 8–10 in Series 1 by Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test after one-way repeated measures ANOVA. (E) Comparison of retention time normalized by that of trials 8–10 in Day 0 between young and aged mice. Mean ± S.E.M.
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Effects of exercise in the first series on the second series of exercise. In the first series (Day 0; left column), mice were placed on the rod rotating at 7 (A; n = 7), 9 (B; n = 9) or 11 (C; n = 6) rpm. In the second series (Day 7; right column), mice were placed on the rod rotating at 9 rpm. Mean ± S.E.M.
Fig. 7
Fig. 7
Effects of lower or higher exercise load on the second series of exercise. (AC) The data in Fig. 5 were grouped into Trials 1–3, 4–7 and 8–10 in Series 1 (Day 0; left column) and Series 2 (Day 7; right column). Mice were placed on the rod rotating at 7 (A; n = 7), 9 (B; n = 9) or 11 (C; n = 6) in series 1, and 9 rpm in series 2. Mean ± S.E.M. Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test was used for statistical analysis, after one-way repeated measures ANOVA. (D) First-order correlation between total retention time (sum of the retention time of trials 1–10) in Series 1 and the retention time of Trials 1–3 in Series 2 (p = 0.0083, n = 22).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Brayne, C. The elephant in the room—healthy brains in later life, epidemiology and public health. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.8, 233–239. 10.1038/nrn2091 (2007). - PubMed
    1. Deary, I. J. et al. Age-associated cognitive decline. Br. Med. Bull.92, 135–152. 10.1093/bmb/ldp033 (2009). - PubMed
    1. Onaolapo, A. Y., Obelawo, A. Y. & Onaolapo, O. J. Brain ageing, cognition and diet: A review of the emerging roles of food-based nootropics in mitigating age-related memory decline. Curr. Aging Sci.12, 2–14. 10.2174/1874609812666190311160754 (2019). - PMC - PubMed
    1. Pennisi, G., Bella, R. & Lanza, G. Motor cortex plasticity in subcortical ischemic vascular dementia: What can TMS say?. Clin. Neurophysiol.126, 851–852. 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.09.001 (2015). - PubMed
    1. Boyden, E. S., Katoh, A. & Raymond, J. L. Cerebellum-dependent learning: The role of multiple plasticity mechanisms. Annu. Rev. Neurosci.27, 581–609. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144238 (2004). - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources