Bayesian active sound localisation: To what extent do humans perform like an ideal-observer?
- PMID: 39774775
- PMCID: PMC11741579
- DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1012108
Bayesian active sound localisation: To what extent do humans perform like an ideal-observer?
Abstract
Self-motion is an essential but often overlooked component of sound localisation. As the directional information of a source is implicitly contained in head-centred acoustic cues, that acoustic input needs to be continuously combined with sensorimotor information about the head orientation in order to decode to a world-centred frame of reference. When utilised, head movements significantly reduce ambiguities in the directional information provided by the incoming sound. In this work, we model human active sound localisation (considering small head rotations) as an ideal observer. In the evaluation, we compared human performance obtained in a free-field active localisation experiment with the predictions of a Bayesian model. Model noise parameters were set a-priori based on behavioural results from other studies, i.e., without any post-hoc parameter fitting to behavioural results. The model predictions showed a general agreement with actual human performance. However, a spatial analysis revealed that the ideal observer was not able to predict localisation behaviour for each source direction. A more detailed investigation into the effects of various model parameters indicated that uncertainty on head orientation significantly contributed to the observed differences. Yet, the biases and spatial distribution of the human responses remained partially unexplained by the presented ideal observer model, suggesting that human sound localisation is sub-optimal.
Copyright: © 2025 McLachlan et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Figures








Similar articles
-
Influence of head position on the spatial representation of acoustic targets.J Neurophysiol. 1999 Jun;81(6):2720-36. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.6.2720. J Neurophysiol. 1999. PMID: 10368392
-
An ideal-observer model of human sound localization.Biol Cybern. 2014 Apr;108(2):169-81. doi: 10.1007/s00422-014-0588-4. Epub 2014 Feb 26. Biol Cybern. 2014. PMID: 24570350
-
Dynamic sound localization during rapid eye-head gaze shifts.J Neurosci. 2004 Oct 20;24(42):9291-302. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2671-04.2004. J Neurosci. 2004. PMID: 15496665 Free PMC article.
-
Absence of compensation for vestibular-evoked passive head rotations in human sound localization.Eur J Neurosci. 2011 Oct;34(7):1149-60. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07844.x. Epub 2011 Sep 6. Eur J Neurosci. 2011. PMID: 21895805
-
[Sound localization cues of binaural hearing].Laryngorhinootologie. 2003 Apr;82(4):240-8. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-38932. Laryngorhinootologie. 2003. PMID: 12717598 Review. German.
References
-
- Wallach H. The role of head movements and vestibular and visual cues in sound localization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1940;27(4):339. doi: 10.1037/h0054629 - DOI
-
- Jiang J, Xie B, Mai H, Liu L, Yi K, Zhang C. The role of dynamic cue in auditory vertical localisation. Applied Acoustics. 2019;146:398–408. doi: 10.1016/j.apacoust.2018.12.002 - DOI
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Research Materials