Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Feb 1;13(1):5.
doi: 10.1186/s40462-025-00531-y.

Spatial and temporal predictability drive foraging movements of coastal birds

Affiliations

Spatial and temporal predictability drive foraging movements of coastal birds

Madeleine Foley et al. Mov Ecol. .

Abstract

Background: Temporal and spatial predictability of food resources are critical to the foraging efficiency of central place foragers. While site fidelity is often assessed in this context, route fidelity, or the repeated use of the same path while traveling, and temporal aspects of habitat predictability have received less attention. We examined how the use of urban, coastal, and offshore habitats influenced spatiotemporal predictability in the foraging patterns of herring gulls (Larus argentatus) and great black-backed gulls (L. marinus). Since gulls show higher site fidelity when foraging in urban habitats, we predicted that these trips would also show higher route fidelity. Similarly, we predicted that gulls foraging in coastal habitats would adapt the timing of foraging trips relative to tides.

Methods: We analyzed GPS tracks of herring gulls (n = 79) and great black-backed gulls (n = 37)-between 2016-2022 from four nesting colonies whose surrounding areas varied in their degree of urbanization. Fréchet distance, which is defined as the repeated use of the same path while traveling, was used to assess route fidelity, within colonies and between habitat types. We also compared the consistency of foraging trip timing relative to tidal stage and day of week, respectively, across habitat types.

Results: Neither herring nor great black-backed gulls showed higher route fidelity in urban habitats. Herring gulls showed direct travel between urban foraging sites but revisited sites in different orders, suggesting that a mosaic map may be used to navigate between known urban foraging sites. Herring and great black-backed gulls that foraged at coastal sites exhibited patterns in trip timing in relation to the tidal cycle, with foraging primarily occurring at or around low tide. Herring gulls in urban environments foraged more on Fridays and weekends, possibly due to increased or altered human activities on these days.

Conclusions: Our results demonstrate the importance of spatial memory and spatiotemporal predictability of gull foraging habitats and highlight the extent to which gulls adjust their movements based on their foraging habitats.

Keywords: Larus argentatus; Larus marinus; Fréchet distance; GPS tracking; Great black-backed gull; Herring gull; Route fidelity; Seabirds; Tidal patterns.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Ethics approval and consent to participate: All animal handling and sampling were approved by Stony Brook University’s institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC number 875550). Consent for publication: Not applicable. Competing interests: The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Location of four study colonies In New York and Massachusetts on the east coast of the United States. The extent of the detailed map is shown within North America on the left
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Fréchet distances for herring gulls (left) and great black-backed gulls (right) foraging in different habitats from AOVs accounting for colony effects. Values reflect means ± standard error. Urban (coastal/offshore) habitats reflect foraging trips in which birds were exclusively foraging in urban (coastal/offshore) habitats, while multiple habitats reflect foraging trips in which birds used multiple habitats. Note that lower Fréchet distances reflect higher route fidelity
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Bar plots and stacked rose plots showing the average proportion of herring gull foraging points (± SE for bar plots) across individuals in each habitat type and tide time, and frequency of occurrence of foraging points in each two-hour tidal period within urban, coastal and offshore habitats across all individuals at Young’s Island, Jamaica Bay, Muskeget and Tuckernuck, respectively. The tidal cycle is broken up into 2-h periods, low tide, flood tide 1, flood tide 2, high tide, ebb tide 1, ebb tide 2
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Bar plots and stacked rose plots showing the average proportion (± SE) of great black-backed gull foraging points across individuals in each habitat type and tide time, and frequency of occurrence of foraging points in each two-hour tidal period within urban, coastal and offshore habitats across all individuals at Young’s Island and Muskeget, respectively. The tidal cycle is broken up into 2-h periods, low tide, flood tide 1, flood tide 2, high tide, ebb tide 1, ebb tide 2
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Example of consecutive foraging trips (numbered 1–10) of a herring gull tracked at the Young’s Island study colony. The extent of the foraging trip plots is shown in the map of Long Island, New York in the upper left of the figure. The tagged bird made repeat visits to several urban foraging spots, shown with colored circles. Foraging areas that were not visited repeatedly are shown in gray. Colony point colored pink. Arrows along tracks show the direction of travel

Similar articles

References

    1. Able KP. The concepts and terminology of bird navigation. J Avian Biol. 2001;32(2):174–83.
    1. Agostinelli C and Lund U. R package 'circular': Circular Statistics (version 0.5–0). 2023. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=circular
    1. Alldredge AL, Hamner WM. Recurring aggregation of zooplankton by a tidal current. Estuar Coast Mar Sci. 1980;10(1):31–7.
    1. Alerstam T, Hake M, Kjellén N. Temporal and spatial patterns of repeated migratory journeys by ospreys. Anim Behav. 2006;71(3):555–66.
    1. Anderson CM, Gilchrist HG, Ronconi RA, Shlepr KR, Clark DE, Fifield DA, Robertson GJ, Mallory ML. Both short and long distance migrants use energy-minimizing migration strategies in North American herring gulls. Movement Ecol. 2020;8(26):1. - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources