Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Apr;87(3):721-727.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-024-03008-z. Epub 2025 Feb 19.

Information-driven attentional capture

Affiliations

Information-driven attentional capture

Alenka Doyle et al. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2025 Apr.

Abstract

Visual attention, the selective prioritization of sensory information, is crucial in dynamic, information-rich environments. That both internal goals and external salience modulate the allocation of attention is well established. However, recent empirical work has found instances of experience-driven attention, wherein task-irrelevant, physically non-salient stimuli reflexively capture attention in ways that are contingent on an observer's unique history. The prototypical example of experience-driven attention relies on a history of reward associations, with evidence attributing the phenomenon to reward-prediction errors. However, a mechanistic account, differing from the reward-prediction error hypothesis, is needed to explain how, in the absence of monetary reward, a history of target-seeking leads to attentional capture. Here we propose that what drives attentional capture in such cases is not target-seeking, but an association with instrumental information. To test this hypothesis, we used pre-cues to render the information provided by a search target either instrumental or redundant. We found that task-irrelevant, physically non-salient distractors associated with instrumental information were more likely to draw eye movements (a sensitive metric of information sampling) than were distractors associated with redundant information. Furthermore, saccading to an instrumental-information-associated distractor led to a greater behavioral cost: response times were slowed more severely. Crucially, the distractors had equivalent histories as sought targets, so any attentional differences between them must be due to different information histories resulting from our experimental manipulation. These findings provide strong evidence for the information history hypothesis and offer a method for exploring the neural signature of information-driven attentional capture.

Keywords: Attention; Attention in learning; Attentional capture.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Conflicts of interest: Nothing to report. Ethics approval: This study received ethics approval by the Trinity College Institutional Review Board. Consent to participate: Informed consent was obtained for each participant. Consent for publication: Not applicable.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Training phase task and information manipulation. Observers have 800 ms to search for a color-defined target and report the orientation of the line inside (horizontal or vertical). Half the trials are pre-cued with reliable information about target color (target matches pre-cue color) and half are pre-cued with unreliable information (target matches one of two pre-cue colors). Training phase results. Proportion correct (top) and mean response time for correct trials (bottom) for each pre-cued condition; each dot is an individual participant; the outcome of a paired t-test is reported in each scatterplot. Test phase task and results. Observers have 1,200 ms to search for the shape singleton and report the orientation of the line inside (horizontal or vertical). Half the trials contain an information-associated distractor (instrumental-information-associated or redundant-information-associated). Dotted lines on posterior distributions indicate 95% credible intervals, the dashed line indicates the change in proportion of saccades calculated directly from the data. See main text for additional details

References

    1. Anderson, B. A. (2017). Reward processing in the value-driven attention network: Reward signals tracking cue identity and location. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,12(3), 461–467. 10.1093/scan/nsw141 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson, B. A., & Halpern, M. (2017). On the value-dependence of value-driven attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,79(4), 1001–1011. 10.3758/s13414-017-1289-6 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson, B. A., & Kim, H. (2019). On the relationship between value-driven and stimulus-driven attentional capture. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,81(3), 607–613. 10.3758/s13414-019-01670-2 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson, B. A., & Yantis, S. (2012). Value-driven attentional and oculomotor capture during goal-directed, unconstrained viewing. Attention Perception & Psychophysics,74(8), 1644–1653. 10.3758/s13414-012-0348-2 - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2011). Value-driven attentional capture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,108(25), 10367–10371. 10.1073/pnas.1104047108 - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources