Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Mar 4;51(Suppl 2):S194-S204.
doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbad183.

Personal Goal-Related Mental Time Travel and Its Association With Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Individuals With High Schizotypal Traits

Affiliations

Personal Goal-Related Mental Time Travel and Its Association With Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Individuals With High Schizotypal Traits

Jun-Yan Ye et al. Schizophr Bull. .

Abstract

Background and hypothesis: Mental time travel (MTT) is a crucial ability for daily life. Personal goal-related MTT events has stronger phenomenological characteristics than personal goal-unrelated ones, ie, the "personal goal-advantage effect". However, it remains unclear whether this effect is impacted in individuals with high schizotypal traits (HST) and the neural correlates of this effect have yet to be elucidated. The present study aimed to fill these knowledge gaps. We hypothesized that HST would show a reduced "personal goal-advantage effect" in MTT and would exhibit altered relationships with resting-state functional connectivity.

Study design: In Study 1, 37 HST and 40 individuals with low schizotypal traits (LST) were recruited. Participants generated MTT events with personal goal-related and personal goal-unrelated cues. In Study 2, 39 HST and 38 LST were recruited, they completed the same behavioral task and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning.

Study results: Both Study 1 and Study 2 revealed that HST exhibited reduced "personal goal-advantage effect" on MTT specificity. Moreover, Study 2 showed that compared with LST, HST exhibited altered association between the "personal goal-advantage effect" and functional connectivity (ie, between the right precuneus and the left postcentral gyrus and "personal goal-advantage effect" on emotional valence, between the left hippocampus and the right temporal fusiform gyrus and "personal goal-advantage effect" on emotional intensity).

Conclusions: These findings suggest that HST exhibit a reduced "personal goal-advantage effect" in MTT specificity and altered neural correlates related to this effect. The "personal goal-advantage effect" may be a potential target for intervention in HST.

Keywords: mental time travel; neural; personal goal-advantage effect; schizotypal.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

None.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Procedure of personal goal mental time travel task.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Significant group difference of the relationship between personal goal-advantage effect on MTT and functional connectivity. DPG_MTT_Valence = The "personal goal-advantage effect" on emotional valence; DPG_MTT_Intensity =The "personal goal-advantage effect" on emotional intensity; rsFC = resting-state functional connectivity; HIP = hippocampus. (A) Non-significant correlation between the functional connectivity of the right precuneus and the left postcentral gyrus with personal goal-advantage effect on emotional valence in LST, but this correlation was positive in HST. (B) The negative correlation between the functional connectivity of the left hippocampus and the right temporal fusiform gyrus with personal goal-advantage effect on emotional intensity in LST, but this correlation was positive in HST. Threshold: cluster-level FWE adjusted P < .05, cluster size >20 (voxel-level uncorrected P < .005), multiple comparison correction PFWEcorrection = .005.

References

    1. Brocas I, Carrillo JD. A neuroeconomic theory of mental time travel. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:658. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00658 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC. The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30(3):299–313; discussion 313. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x07001975 - DOI - PubMed
    1. D’Argembeau A, Renaud O, Van der Linden M. Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2011;25(1):96–103. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1647 - DOI
    1. Schacter DL, Benoit RG, De Brigard F, Szpunar KK. Episodic future thinking and episodic counterfactual thinking: intersections between memory and decisions. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;117:14–21. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.12.008 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Schacter DL, Madore KP. Remembering the past and imagining the future: identifying and enhancing the contribution of episodic memory. Mem Stud. 2016;9(3):245–255. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698016645230 - DOI - PMC - PubMed