Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 May-Jun;30(3):e70021.
doi: 10.1111/infa.70021.

Evidence of Cross-Cultural Differences in Maternal Mind-Mindedness

Affiliations

Evidence of Cross-Cultural Differences in Maternal Mind-Mindedness

Fabiola Silletti et al. Infancy. 2025 May-Jun.

Abstract

Cross-cultural research on maternal mind-mindedness- the proclivity to view the child as a mental agent-can enhance our understanding of caregiving determinants and children's social-cognitive variations across cultures. However, cross-cultural studies on mind-mindedness remain limited. To address this gap, we examined mothers' use of appropriate (AMRCs) and non-attuned (NAMRCs) mind-related comments in Italy (N = 88), Germany (N = 64), and the Netherlands (N = 97) with their 12-month-old infants (N = 249; 133 girls and 116 boys). Cluster analysis revealed three maternal profiles: low use of both AMRCs and NAMRCs, high use of both AMRCs and NAMRCs, and high AMRCs with low NAMRCs. Almost half of the German mothers belonged to the first profile, most Italian mothers to the second, and Dutch mothers were equally distributed across the three. These findings highlight, for the first time, cultural influences on maternal mind-mindedness within Western countries and emphasize the need to move beyond a simplistic West-East comparison, recognizing that cultural differences can be observed even within similar contexts, and call for culturally sensitive psychoeducational interventions to enhance caregivers' mentalizing skills.

Keywords: cross‐cultural studies; infancy; infant‐caregiver interaction; mentalization; mind‐mindedness.

PubMed Disclaimer

References

    1. Aldrich, N. J., J. Chen, and L. Alfieri. 2021. “Evaluating Associations Between Parental Mind‐Mindedness and Children’s Developmental Capacities Through Meta‐Analysis.” Developmental Review 60: 100946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2021.100946.
    1. Axia, G., and T. S. Weisner. 2002. “Infant Stress Reactivity and Home Cultural Ecology of Italian Infants and Families.” Infant Behavior and Development 25, no. 3: 255–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163‐6383(02)00099‐1.
    1. Bernier, A., C. A. McMahon, and R. Perrier. 2017. “Maternal Mind‐Mindedness and Children’s School Readiness: A Longitudinal Study of Developmental Processes.” Developmental Psychology 53, no. 2: 210–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000225.
    1. Best, D. L., A. S. House, A. E. Barnard, and B. S. Spicker. 1994. “Parent‐Child Interactions in France, Germany, and Italy: The Effects of Gender and Culture.” Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology 25, no. 2: 181–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022194252002.
    1. Bornstein, M. H., and C. S. L. Cheah. 2006. “The Place of ‘Culture and Parenting’ in the Ecological Contextual Perspective on Developmental Science.” In Parenting Beliefs, Behaviors, and Parent‐Child Relations: A Cross‐Cultural Perspective, edited by K. H. Rubin and O. B. Chung, 3–33. Psychology Press.

LinkOut - more resources