Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 May 16;17(1):26.
doi: 10.1186/s11689-025-09607-7.

Visual feedback and motor memory contributions to sustained motor control deficits in autism spectrum disorder across childhood and into adulthood

Affiliations

Visual feedback and motor memory contributions to sustained motor control deficits in autism spectrum disorder across childhood and into adulthood

Robin L Shafer et al. J Neurodev Disord. .

Abstract

Background: Autistic individuals show deficits in sustained fine motor control which are associated with an over-reliance on visual feedback. Motor memory deficits also have been reported during sustained fine motor control in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The development of motor memory and visuomotor feedback processes contributing to sustained motor control issues in ASD are not known. The present study aimed to characterize age-related changes in visual feedback and motor memory processes contributing to sustained fine motor control issues in ASD.

Methods: Fifty-four autistic participants and 31 neurotypical (NT) controls ages 10-25 years completed visually guided and memory guided sustained precision gripping tests by pressing on force sensors with their dominant hand index finger and thumb. For visually guided trials, participants viewed a stationary target bar and a force bar that moved upwards with increased force for 15s. During memory guided trials, the force bar was visible for 3s, after which participants attempted to maintain their force output without visual feedback for another 12s. To assess visual feedback processing, force accuracy, variability (standard deviation), and regularity (sample entropy) were examined. To assess motor memory, force decay latency, slope, and magnitude were examined during epochs without visual feedback.

Results: Relative to NT controls, autistic individuals showed a greater magnitude and a trend for a steeper slope of force decay during memory guided trials. Across conditions, the ASD group showed reduced force accuracy (β = 0.41, R2 = 0.043, t79.3=2.36, p = .021) and greater force variability (β=-2.16, R2 = 0.143, t77.1=-4.04, p = .0001) and regularity (β=-0.52, R2 = 0.021, t77.4=-2.21, p = .030) relative to NT controls at younger ages, but these differences normalized by adolescence (age x group interactions). Lower force accuracy and greater force variability during visually guided trials and steeper decay slope during memory guided trials were associated with overall autism severity.

Conclusions: Our findings that autistic individuals show a greater magnitude and tendency for a greater rate of force decay than NT individuals following the removal of visual feedback indicate that motor memory deficits contribute to fine motor control issues in ASD. Findings that sensorimotor differences in ASD were specific to younger ages suggest delayed development across multiple motor control processes.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorders; Entropy; Fine motor control; Grip force; Motor memory; Sensorimotor; Sensory integration; Visual feedback; Visuomotor.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Ethics approval and consent to participate: Adult participants provided written informed consent after a complete description of the study, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. For participants under the age of 18 and adults who were under legal guardianship, a parent or legal guardian provided written informed consent on behalf of the participant, and the participant provided written assent. All study procedures were approved by the University of Kansas Medical Center Institutional Review Board (IRB#: STUDY00140269). Consent for publication: Not applicable. Competing interests: MWM is PI on an investigator initiated clinical trial of behavioral inflexibility in autism funded by Acadia Pharmaceuticals. MWM and ZW received funding from Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation for an investigator-initiated study of Phelan McDermid Syndrome. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Task design. (A) During visually guided trials, participants see a target bar that turns from yellow to green to indicate that they should start pressing. Participants also view feedback of their force output (white bar) for the entire trial. (B) During memory guided trials, participants see visual feedback of their force output (white bar) and the green target bar for the first 3s of the trial, after which the white force bar disappears, and they are instructed to keep pressing at the same force level until the target turns red (12s later). (C) Example force output (dark blue) in Newtons (N) for a neurotypical participant during a visually guided trial. The grey line represents target force. (D) Example force output (dark blue) in Newtons (N) for a neurotypical participant during a memory guided trial with target force indicated by the grey line. The participants’ force usually begins to decay (black arrow) after the visual feedback disappears. (E) Example force output (dark blue) in Newtons (N) for an autistic participant during a visually guided trial. (F) Example force output (dark blue) in Newtons (N) for an autistic participant during a memory guided trial
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Force accuracy. Age (years; log10 scale) associations with force accuracy (proportion of mean force to target force) for (A) the autism (ASD; red circles) and (B) the neurotypical (NT; blue triangles) groups. The regression lines represent the significant group x age effects (across conditions), though data for visually guided (Vis; solid points) and memory guided (Mem; empty points) are also shown. (C) Force accuracy depicted as a function of group (NT: blue triangles; ASD: red circles) during visually guided (Vis; filled points) and memory guided (Mem; empty points) precision gripping. The large points represent group x condition marginal means adjusted for random intercepts of subject in the LMER models. The * represents the significant condition main effect. Error bands (A, B) and bars (C) represent the 95% confidence intervals from the MLM models after accounting for random intercepts of participant
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Force Variability in Newtons (N). Age (years; log10 scale) associations with force standard deviation in Newtons (N; log10 scale) for (A) the autism (ASD; red circles) and (B) neurotypical control (NT; blue triangles) groups. The regression lines represent the significant group x age effects (across conditions), though data for visually guided (Vis; solid points) and memory guided (Mem; empty points) are also shown. (C) Force variability depicted as a function of group (NT: blue triangles; ASD: red circles) during visually guided (Vis; filled points) and memory guided (Mem; empty points) precision gripping. The large points represent group x condition marginal means adjusted for random intercepts of subject in the LMER models. The * represents the significant group main effect. Error bands (A, B) and bars (C) represent the 95% confidence intervals from the MLM models after accounting for random intercepts of participant
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Force regularity. Age (years; log10 scale) associations with force SampEn (unitless; log10 scale) for (A) the autism (ASD; red circles) and (B) neurotypical control (NT; blue triangles) groups. Higher SampEn corresponds to lower regularity. The regression lines represent the significant group x age effects (across conditions), though data for visually guided (Vis; solid points) and memory guided (Mem; empty points) are also shown. (C) Force regularity depicted as a function of group (NT: blue triangles; ASD: red circles) during the visually guided feedback (Vis; solid points) and memory guided (Mem; empty points) conditions. The large points represent group x condition marginal means adjusted for random intercepts of subject in the LMER models. The * represents the significant effect of group in the visually guided condition (group x condition interaction). Error bands (A, B) and bars (C) represent the 95% confidence intervals from the MLM models after accounting for random intercepts of subject
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Decay slope and magnitude. (A) Slope of the force decay (square root scale) following the removal of visual feedback (memory guided condition) for the autism (ASD; red circles) and neurotypical control (NT; blue triangles) groups. (B) Magnitude of the force decay (log10 scale) following the removal of visual feedback (memory guided condition) for the ASD (red circles) and NT control (blue triangles) groups. Large points represent group means adjusted for random intercepts of subject in the MLM models. The * represents a significant effect of group. Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals from the MLM models after accounting for random intercepts of subject
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Relation of ASD Symptomatology to grip control. Association between Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Composite Severity Scores (ADOS-CSS) for the autism group (ASD; red circles) and (A) force accuracy (proportion of mean force to target force) and (B) force variability (standard deviation; log10 scale) in Newtons (N) during visually guided (Vis; solid points) precision gripping. (C) Association between ADOS-CSS scores for the ASD group (red circles) and the slope of logarithmic force decay (square root scale) during memory guided precision gripping (Mem; empty points). Error bands represent standard error

Update of

Similar articles

References

    1. Jong MD, Punt M, Groot ED, Minderaa RB, Hadders-Algra M. Minor neurological dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2011;53(7):641–6. - PubMed
    1. Estes A, Zwaigenbaum L, Gu H, St. John T, Paterson S, Elison JT, et al. Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7(1):24. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hannant P, Cassidy S, Tavassoli T, Mann F. Sensorimotor Difficulties Are Associated with the Severity of Autism Spectrum Conditions. Front Integr Neurosci. 2016;10. Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Article/10.3389/fnint.2016.00028/abstract - PMC - PubMed
    1. Haswell CC, Izawa J, Dowell LR, Mostofsky SH, Shadmehr R. Representation of internal models of action in the autistic brain. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(8):970–2. - PMC - PubMed
    1. LeBarton ES, Iverson JM. Fine motor skill predicts expressive Language in infant siblings of children with autism. Dev Sci. 2013;16(6):815–27. - PMC - PubMed