Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Jun 10;80(7):gbaf081.
doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaf081.

Patterns of Perceived Control That Buffer Against Cognitive Decline in Midlife and Old Age

Affiliations

Patterns of Perceived Control That Buffer Against Cognitive Decline in Midlife and Old Age

Jeremy M Hamm et al. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. .

Abstract

Objectives: The relationship between domain-general or global perceptions of control and cognition is well-established. However, little is known about how these domain-general beliefs combine with domain-specific perceptions in central life domains to form multifaceted patterns of control that may buffer against cognitive declines in midlife and old age.

Methods: We used 9-year data from the Midlife in the United States Study (n = 2,734, Mage = 55 years, range = 33-83; 58% female) to identify profiles of domain-general (personal mastery, perceived constraints) and domain-specific control over central life domains (health, work, finances, others' welfare, child relationships, and partner relationship). We subsequently assessed profile differences in 9-year trajectories of cognitive aging and whether these differences became pronounced in old age.

Results: Factor mixture models showed that 4 common profiles emerged: low control, family control, work control, and domain-specific control. Autoregressive ANCOVAs showed the family control and work control profiles experienced the least 9-year decline in executive functioning (F3,2330 = 3.46, p = .016). Moderation models showed the family control profile experienced less decline in executive functioning than the work control profile, but only in old age (b = -0.006, p = .020). Supplemental analyses showed profile differences in cognitive aging were (a) mediated by theory-derived process variables (positive and negative affect) and (b) extended to a broader suite of health-related developmental outcomes (functional limitations, chronic conditions, and mortality).

Discussion: Findings inform lifespan theories of development by documenting meaningful patterns of domain-general and domain-specific control that have implications for healthy cognitive aging.

Keywords: Cognitive aging; Control belief profiles; Domain-specific; Latent profile analysis; Person-centered approach.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

None.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Results from the k = 4 profile model of domain-specific and domain-general perceived control. The low control profile (n = 670) reflected individuals with very low levels of control across all aspects of domain-specific and domain-general control. The family control profile (n = 663) reflected individuals with high levels of control over their family relationships with children and spouses and relatively average levels of control over most other aspects (with above-average levels of mastery). The work control profile (n = 793) had higher levels of control over work and finances, lower levels of control over family relationships with children and spouses, and above-average levels of mastery and below-average levels of control. The high domain-specific control profile (n = 608) had high levels of control over all aspects of domain-specific control but had average levels of mastery and constraints.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Contextualized effect sizes of profile differences on regressed change in executive functioning. Estimates suggested rates of 9-year decline in executive functioning were respectively reduced by approximately 22% and 24% for the family control profile versus the low control and domain-specific control profiles. Predicted values were adjusted for model covariates and for (raw) average sample declines of −0.256 units in executive functioning. 22% reduction in rate of decline. ††24% reduction in rate of decline.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Age × Work Control interaction predicting 9-year regressed change in executive functioning. Predicted values were adjusted for model covariates and for (raw) average sample declines of −0.256 units in executive functioning.

Similar articles

References

    1. Agrigoroaei, S., & Lachman, M. E. (2011). Cognitive functioning in midlife and old age: Combined effects of psychosocial and behavioral factors. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 66B(Suppl. 1), i130–i140. https://doi.org/ 10.1093/geronb/gbr017 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Brim, O. G., Ryff, C. D., & Kessler, R. C. (2004). How healthy are we?: A national study of well-being at midlife. University of Chicago Press.
    1. Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312(5782), 1913–1915. https://doi.org/ 10.1126/science.1127488 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Carstensen, L. L., & Hershfield, H. E. (2021). Beyond stereotypes: Using socioemotional selectivity theory to improve messaging to older adults. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 30(4), 327–334. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/09637214211011468 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. The American Psychologist, 54(3), 165–181. https://doi.org/ 10.1037//0003-066x.54.3.165 - DOI - PubMed