Tulving's (1989) Doctrine of Concordance Revisited
- PMID: 40417260
- PMCID: PMC12101318
- DOI: 10.5334/joc.447
Tulving's (1989) Doctrine of Concordance Revisited
Abstract
The Doctrine of Concordance is the implicit assumption that cognitive processes, behavior, and phenomenological experience are highly correlated (Tulving, 1989). Tulving challenged this assumption, pointing to domains in which conscious experience did not accompany a particular measured cognitive process and to situations in which consciousness did not correlate with the observable behavior. Schwartz (1999) extended this view, asserting that the underlying cognitive processes that produce conscious experience may differ from those that produce observable behavior. Though research on conscious experience blossomed during the last quarter century and progress has been made in moving past the Doctrine of Concordance, we argue that some subdomains within memory research remain hampered by an implicit endorsement of it. We outline two areas of memory research in which current research and interpretations appear to fall prey to the Doctrine today: research on the dual- vs. single-process theory in recognition memory, including work on remember/know judgments, and research on retrospective memory confidence. We then describe four areas of research that show progress in understanding conscious experience by rejecting the Doctrine of Concordance: These are 1) metacognitive disconnects in the science of learning, 2) recognition illusions, 3) déjà vu experiences, and 4) aha experiences. We claim that there is often a dissociation between the mechanisms that create conscious experience and the underlying cognitive processes that contribute to behaviors, which may seem causally correlated with conscious experience. Disentangling the relations between process, behavior, and conscious experience in the human mind's operation are important to understanding it.
Keywords: Doctrine of Concordance; confidence; conscious experience; consciousness; dual process theories of recognition; déjà vu experience; experience; metacognition; remember/know judgments; retrospective confidence; subjective experience.
Copyright: © 2025 The Author(s).
Conflict of interest statement
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
Figures


Similar articles
-
Sparkling at the end of the tongue: the etiology of tip-of-the-tongue phenomenology.Psychon Bull Rev. 1999 Sep;6(3):379-93. doi: 10.3758/bf03210827. Psychon Bull Rev. 1999. PMID: 12198776 Review.
-
Opening a conceptual space for metamemory experience.New Ideas Psychol. 2023 Apr;69:100995. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2022.100995. Epub 2022 Nov 19. New Ideas Psychol. 2023. PMID: 38223256 Free PMC article.
-
The radiation of autonoetic consciousness in cognitive neuroscience: A functional neuroanatomy perspective.Neuropsychologia. 2020 Jun;143:107477. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107477. Epub 2020 Apr 30. Neuropsychologia. 2020. PMID: 32360475 Review.
-
Psychodynamic Therapy.2024 Sep 2. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2025 Jan–. 2024 Sep 2. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2025 Jan–. PMID: 39163451 Free Books & Documents.
-
The relation of subjective experience to cognitive processing.Behav Brain Sci. 2023 Nov 14;46:e375. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X23000262. Behav Brain Sci. 2023. PMID: 37961777
References
-
- Abbott, E. E. (1909). On the analysis of the factor of recall in the learning process. The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 11(1), 159–177. 10.1037/h0093018 - DOI
-
- Baddeley, A. D., & Longman, D. J. (1978). The influence of length and frequency of training session on the rate of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21(8), 627–635. 10.1080/00140137808931764 - DOI
Publication types
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Miscellaneous