Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 May 9;15(5):644.
doi: 10.3390/bs15050644.

Metacognitive Prompts Influence 7- to 9-Year-Olds' Executive Function at the Levels of Task Performance and Neural Processing

Affiliations

Metacognitive Prompts Influence 7- to 9-Year-Olds' Executive Function at the Levels of Task Performance and Neural Processing

Colin Drexler et al. Behav Sci (Basel). .

Abstract

To elucidate the role of metacognitive reflection in the development of children's executive function (EF) skills, the current study examined relations among implicit and explicit forms of metacognition in 7- to 9-year-olds during performance based on the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS), while experimentally manipulating the propensity to reflect on the task. Results showed that instructions to reflect led to improved task accuracy and better metacognitive control, but only in younger children, likely because older children were already engaging in reflection. Individual differences in trait mindfulness were related to a similarly reflective mode of responding, characterized by improved task accuracy and metacognitive control. In contrast, articulatory suppression impaired children's task accuracy and metacognitive monitoring. Additionally, simply asking children to make metacognitive judgments without extra instructions decreased the amplitude of event-related potential (ERP) indices of error detection (the error-related negativity; ERN) and conflict detection (the N2). Finally, individual differences in trait anxiety were related to larger Pe amplitudes. Taken together, the current findings reinforce theoretical frameworks integrating metacognition and EF and highlight the shared influence of metacognitive reflection across multiple levels of analysis.

Keywords: ERN; N2; error-monitoring; event-related potentials; executive function; metacognition; post-error slowing; reflection.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Figures

Figure A1
Figure A1
Schematic depicting the timeline of a trial for the metacognitive DCCS, with MC monitoring and MC control questions that appeared every 2–5 trials in a pseudo-random order.
Figure A2
Figure A2
Overall internal consistency reliability across conditions for the ERN, Pe, and N2 components’ mean amplitudes. The error bars represent 95% confidence intervals from the resampling distribution. The red dotted line represents the threshold for acceptable data quality (0.60), the black solid line represents the threshold for good data quality (0.80), and the black dotted line represents the threshold for excellent data quality (0.90).
Figure A3
Figure A3
Averaged standardized measurement error (aSME) output across conditions for the ERN, Pe, and N2 components. The error bars represent 95% confidence intervals of the variability between individuals. Dots represent the individual participants. aSME = Analytic Standard Measurement Error.
Figure 1
Figure 1
(a) DCCS accuracy by experimental condition; (b) simple slopes of the interaction between reflection prompts and age based on DCCS accuracy. An asterisk (*) indicates that the slope is significantly different from zero (p < 0.05).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Metacognitive monitoring by experimental condition.
Figure 3
Figure 3
(a) Metacognitive control score by experimental condition; (b) simple slopes of the interaction between reflection prompts and age based on the metacognitive control score. An asterisk (*) indicates that the slope is significantly different from zero (p < 0.05).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Response-locked ERPs (ERN and Pe components) by experimental condition. Shaded areas indicate the post-response temporal regions during which the ERN (top panel) and the Pe (bottom panel) were measured. Participants in the MC Only condition displayed a significantly smaller (i.e., less negative) ERN amplitude than those in the No MC condition (β = 0.67, SE = 0.31, p = 0.03), reflecting the attenuating influence of metacognitive questions on the ERN amplitude.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Stimulus-locked ERPs (the N2 component) by experimental condition. Shaded areas indicate the post-response temporal regions during which the N2 was measured. Participants in the MC Only condition demonstrated significantly smaller (i.e., less negative) N2 amplitudes than children in the No MC condition (β = 0.93, SE = 0.29, p = 0.002), reflecting the attenuating influence of metacognitive questions on the N2 amplitude.

References

    1. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology. 1995;57(1):289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. - DOI
    1. Birmaher B., Khetarpal S., Brent D., Cully M., Balach L., Kaufman J., Neer S. M. The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):545–553. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Boen R., Quintana D. S., Ladouceur C. D., Tamnes C. K. Age-related differences in the error-related negativity and error positivity in children and adolescents are moderated by sample and methodological characteristics: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology. 2022;59(6):e14003. doi: 10.1111/psyp.14003. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bryce D., Whitebread D., Szűcs D. The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7 year-old children. Metacognition and Learning. 2015;10(2):181–198. doi: 10.1007/s11409-014-9120-4. - DOI
    1. Chevalier N., Martis S. B., Curran T., Munakata Y. Metacognitive processes in executive control development: The case of reactive and proactive control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015;27(6):1125–1136. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00782. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources