Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 May 28.
doi: 10.3758/s13420-025-00677-0. Online ahead of print.

Maximizing within-session stability in individual differences during an experiential impulsivity task

Affiliations

Maximizing within-session stability in individual differences during an experiential impulsivity task

Michael E Young et al. Learn Behav. .

Abstract

Behavioral measures of impulsivity and other traits often show weaker test-retest reliability than self-report measures. Weaker reliability impacts the assessment of individual differences in the trait or state being assessed. Behavioral tasks demonstrate greater sensitivity to state variables which may be a key reason for changes in ranked performance across time. The present study examines a single impulsivity task, the escalating interest task, and considers the design principles that may alter the within-session stability of the assessed behavior. A reanalysis of existing data is contrasted with new behavioral data to reveal that rapid changes in task contingencies produced more stable individual differences than prolonged exposure to each contingency. This outcome may be driven by expanding the number of contingencies experienced at each assessment or by keeping behavior in transition. An attempt to avoid floor or ceiling effects by increasing the ambiguity of the contingency, however, did not produce the desired result. The implications of these results for the escalating interest task as well as other behavioral tasks are considered.

Keywords: Behavioral assessment; Escalating interest task; Impulsivity; Reliability; Stability.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Ethics approval: Approval was obtained from Kansas State IRB; the research was classified as exempt. Consent to participate: Informed consent to participate was obtained from each participant in accord with the Kansas State IRB Consent for publication: Consent to publish their results was obtained from each participant in accord with the Kansas State IRB. Conflicts of interest/Competing interests: The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article and no financial or proprietary interests in any material discussed in this article.

Similar articles

References

    1. Ainslie, G. W. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–396. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 - DOI
    1. Barratt, E. S., & Patton, J. H. (1983). Impulsivity: Cognitive, behavioral and psychophysiological correlates. In M. Zuckerman (Ed.), Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and anxiety (pp. 77–122). Lawrence Erlbaum.
    1. Bates, D. M. (2010). lme4: Mixed-effects modeling with R. In (pp. 1–145): Springer.
    1. Buelow, M. T., & Barnhart, W. R. (2018). Test–retest reliability of common behavioral decision making tasks. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 33(1), 125–129. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources