An examination of how reward associations facilitate and impair Stroop performance
- PMID: 40439745
- DOI: 10.1007/s00426-025-02135-y
An examination of how reward associations facilitate and impair Stroop performance
Abstract
Rewarded stimuli are prioritized by the attentional system. Behavioral performance is improved when the task-relevant dimension is tied to a potential reward but is impaired when the irrelevant dimension is reward related. Within the rewarded Stroop task, the facilitation (reward responsiveness) and impairment (modulation of interference of reward association; MIRA) from reward-associated stimuli are thought to be due to different cognitive processes. In four experiments, we explored whether reward responsiveness and MIRA were influenced by reward magnitude and persisted following reward discontinuation. We manipulated how informed participants were of the stimulus-reward contingency based on whether they received stimulus-reward color instructions and whether or not the stimulus-reward contingency was certain (i.e., one color was always tied to one reward outcome). Results suggest that greater reward magnitude increased reward responsiveness, especially when participants were informed about the stimulus-reward contingency. However, greater impairment (MIRA) by a large versus small reward related color word was only observed when participants had little knowledge of the reward contingency (i.e., no instructions and a more uncertain mapping of stimuli to rewards) or during the extinction phase when reward associated colors were less relevant. These findings highlight the distinction between reward responsiveness to maximize gains and the unintentional prioritization of related but irrelevant information and suggest that reward associations that elicit greater reward responsiveness do not necessarily lead to greater impairment of conflict processing.
© 2025. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature.
Conflict of interest statement
Declarations. Ethical approval: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Consent to participate: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Competing interests: The authors declare no competing interests.
Similar articles
-
Drugs for preventing postoperative nausea and vomiting in adults after general anaesthesia: a network meta-analysis.Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020 Oct 19;10(10):CD012859. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012859.pub2. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020. PMID: 33075160 Free PMC article.
-
Education support services for improving school engagement and academic performance of children and adolescents with a chronic health condition.Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2023 Feb 8;2(2):CD011538. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011538.pub2. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2023. PMID: 36752365 Free PMC article.
-
Eliciting adverse effects data from participants in clinical trials.Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018 Jan 16;1(1):MR000039. doi: 10.1002/14651858.MR000039.pub2. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018. PMID: 29372930 Free PMC article.
-
Antiemetics for adults for prevention of nausea and vomiting caused by moderately or highly emetogenic chemotherapy: a network meta-analysis.Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2021 Nov 16;11(11):CD012775. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012775.pub2. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2021. PMID: 34784425 Free PMC article.
-
Antioxidants for male subfertility.Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2022 May 4;5(5):CD007411. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007411.pub5. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2022. PMID: 35506389 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Albertella, L., Copeland, J., Pearson, D., Watson, P., Wiers, R. W., & Le Pelley, M. E. (2017). Selective attention moderates the relationship between attentional capture by signals of nondrug reward and illicit drug use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 175, 99–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.01.041 - DOI - PubMed
-
- Albertella, L., Watson, P., Yücel, M., & Le Pelley, M. E. (2019). Persistence of value-modulated attentional capture is associated with risky alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 10, 100195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100195 - DOI - PubMed - PMC
-
- Anderson, B. A. (2015). Value-driven attentional priority is context specific. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 22, 750–756. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0724-0 - DOI - PubMed - PMC
-
- Anderson, B. A. (2016). The attention habit: How reward learning shapes attentional selection. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369(1), 24–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12957 - DOI - PubMed
-
- Anderson, B. A. (2019). Neurobiology of value-driven attention. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 27–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.11.004 - DOI - PubMed
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources