Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
[Preprint]. 2025 Jul 2:2025.04.21.649913.
doi: 10.1101/2025.04.21.649913.

Aperiodic neural timescales in prefrontal cortex dilate with increased task abstraction

Affiliations

Aperiodic neural timescales in prefrontal cortex dilate with increased task abstraction

Dillan Cellier et al. bioRxiv. .

Abstract

Navigating everyday environments requires that the brain perform information processing at multiple different timescales. For example, while watching a movie we use sensory information from every video frame to construct the current movie scene, which itself is continuously integrated into the narrative arc of the film. This critical function is supported by sensory inputs propagating from dynamic sensory cortices to association cortices, where neural activity remains more stable over time. The hierarchical organization of cortex is therefore reflected in a gradient of neural timescales. While this propagation of inputs up the cortical hierarchy is facilitated by both rhythmic (oscillatory) and non-rhythmic (aperiodic) neural activity, traditional measures of oscillations are often confounded by the influence of aperiodic signals. The reverse is also true: traditional measures of aperiodic neural timescales are influenced by oscillations. This makes it difficult to distinguish between oscillatory and timescale effects in cognition. Here, we analyzed electroencephalography (EEG) data from participants performing a cognitive control task that manipulated the amount of task-relevant contextual information, called task abstraction. Critically, we separated aperiodic neural timescales from the confounding influence of oscillatory power. We hypothesized that neural timescales would increase during the task, and more so in high-abstraction conditions. We found that task abstraction dilated the aperiodic neural timescale, as estimated from the autocorrelation function, over prefrontal cortical regions. Our findings suggests that neural timescales are a dynamic feature of the cerebral cortex that change to meet task demands.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing interests The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1:
Figure 1:. Task Schematic and EEG Analyses
A) Task schematic. This block-design task included 4 conditions that varied along two dimensions: difficulty and abstraction. In the R4 condition (low difficulty, low abstraction) participants had to respond with a button press corresponding to the color they observed on each trial. This required participants to keep in mind a set of 4 stimulus-response mappings throughout the course of the block. The R8 condition (high difficulty, low abstraction) had the same design as R4, except that participants needed to hold 8 stimulus-response mappings in mind over the course of the block. In the D1 condition (low difficulty, high abstraction), participants were making a match-nonmatch judgement on every trial. The two stimuli they were evaluating could vary in both shape and texture. Which feature was relevant for the match-nonmatch judgment was cued by the color of the square surrounding the items. In the D1 condition, the cued color corresponded to the same feature for the entirety of the block. However, in the D2 condition (high difficulty, high abstraction), the cued feature could change trial-to-trial. This meant that on every trial, the participant needed to recall which feature the color of the square cued, evaluate whether the stimuli matched on that feature dimension, and press a button for “match” or “nonmatch.” B) Barplots of the participant-averaged reaction times for each condition, with individual reaction times plotted in grey on top. The high difficulty, high abstraction condition D2 had the longest reaction time in both datasets. C) One common method for estimating neural timescales entails calculating the ACF of a timeseries, and then identifying the lag (x-axis value) at which the ACF crosses a given correlation threshold, N (in this example, N=0.50). This gives a relative measure between timeseries, illustrated in E, where slower signals have larger N-crossing values. This metric will correlate well with the decay rate of the ACF, but mixes the contributions of aperiodic and oscillatory activity. D) An overview of the two procedures used to parameterize the ACF and PSD. Each toolbox (Timescales-methods and FOOOF) produces both aperiodic and oscillatory parameters. The Timescales-methods toolbox for ACF parameterization derives a Tau parameter, which captures the decay rate of the aperiodic component of the ACF. It can also model the oscillatory component of the ACF using a damped cosine function, producing the parameter Cos. The FOOOF toolbox for PSD parameterization produces several parameters related to the aperiodic component of the signal, among them the knee parameter and the exponent parameter. Though the knee parameter most directly relates to the ACF-Tau, it is not reliably visible in real neural data—especially that which is derived from non-invasive imaging. The exponent parameter, which describes the slope of the aperiodic signal, covaries with the knee.
Figure 2:
Figure 2:. Traditional measures of neural timescales show timescale dilation between eyes-open resting state, pre-stimulus task periods, and post-stimulus task periods.
A) Distribution of neural timescales over electrodes during eyes-open and eyes-closed resting state, from Dataset 1. B) Eyes-open resting state exhibits the fastest timescales, compared to pre- and post-stimulus windows, as measured by the 50-Crossing metric. Each point on the boxplot is the trial-averaged data for one participant, for each region of interest. C) (left) Trial-averaged logged 50-Crossing values, sampled from raw ACFs, lengthen pre-to-post stimulus in all conditions, with a main effect of both difficulty and abstraction, as well as an interaction between these (right). The 50-Crossing mixes both aperiodic and oscillatory activity. D) Same as C, but for the posterior channel cluster. The 50-Crossing here shows a significant main effect of condition, which seems to be driven by timescales lengthening selectively during high-difficulty conditions.
Figure 3:
Figure 3:. Aperiodic-Corrected Theta Oscillatory Power Increases with Task Difficulty, with Concurrent Decrease in Alpha Oscillatory Power
A) Frontal theta aperiodic-corrected bandpower (4–8 Hz) increases pre-to-post stimulus, with a main effect of Difficulty revealed by a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA. B) Posterior alpha aperiodic-corrected bandpower (8–12 Hz) decreases in the post-stimulus time window, relative to the pre-stimulus window. This alpha bandpower decrease showed a main effect of both abstraction and difficulty, as well as an interaction between these.
Figure 4:
Figure 4:. Aperiodic Timescales in Prefrontal Electrodes Lengthen with Abstraction
A) Timescales measured from trial-averaged logged ACF Tau parameters (left) lengthen the most in the high-Difficulty, high-Abstraction condition. ACF Tau lengthening (right) from pre-to-post stimulus shows a significant main effect of Abstraction, no main effect of Difficulty, and a significant interaction between Abstraction and Difficulty in a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA. B) Though they increase overall post-stimulus, the ACF Tau parameter measured at Posterior electrodes does not exhibit a condition-specific increase pre-to-post stimulus.

Similar articles

References

    1. Shinn M, Hu A, Turner L, Noble S, Preller KH, Ji JL, et al. Functional brain networks reflect spatial and temporal autocorrelation. Nat Neurosci. 2023. May;26(5):867–78. - PubMed
    1. Buzsáki G, Logothetis N, Singer W. Scaling Brain Size, Keeping Timing: Evolutionary Preservation of Brain Rhythms. Neuron. 2013. Oct 30;80(3):751–64. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Chaudhuri R, Knoblauch K, Gariel MA, Kennedy H, Wang XJ. A Large-Scale Circuit Mechanism for Hierarchical Dynamical Processing in the Primate Cortex. Neuron. 2015. Oct 21;88(2):419–31. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Demirtaş M, Burt JB, Helmer M, Ji JL, Adkinson BD, Glasser MF, et al. Hierarchical Heterogeneity across Human Cortex Shapes Large-Scale Neural Dynamics. Neuron. 2019. Mar 20;101(6):1181–1194.e13. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Huntenburg JM, Bazin PL, Margulies DS. Large-Scale Gradients in Human Cortical Organization. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018. Jan 1;22(1):21–31. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources