Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2024 Jul 23;5(2):e12264.
doi: 10.1002/jcv2.12264. eCollection 2025 Jun.

Special educational needs provision and academic outcomes for children with teacher reported language difficulties at school entry

Affiliations

Special educational needs provision and academic outcomes for children with teacher reported language difficulties at school entry

Sarah Griffiths et al. JCPP Adv. .

Abstract

Background: Language ability predicts academic attainment across the curriculum. Teacher report of language difficulties may therefore help schools identify children that require Special Educational Needs (SEN) provision. Special Educational Needs provision is intended to enable children to reach their academic potential, however the effectiveness of this for children with language difficulties is unknown.

Methods: We linked teacher-ratings on a brief language difficulties questionnaire (13-item) collected in the first year of primary school (N = 7013), with data on SEN provision until age 12-13 and scores on statutory assessments at ages 5-6, 6-7 and 10-11 years from the National Pupil Database (UK). We conducted a preregistered analysis to (a) test the association between teacher-reported language difficulties and later academic outcomes, (b) identify predictors of subsequent SEN provision for monolingual children with language difficulties and (c) test whether SEN provision is associated with better academic outcomes for these children.

Results: Teacher-reported language difficulties predicted achievement in phonics (rs > 0.41), reading (rs > 0.38), writing (rs > 0.32) and maths (rs > 0.40) assessments up to 7 years later. For those with language difficulties, having an existing diagnosis of a neurodevelopmental condition or sensory impairment was the strongest predictor of SEN registration (OR [95% CI] 8.33 [4.12, 19.24]) and special education placement (OR [95% CI] 18.89 [9.29, 42.01]) during primary school. However, 38% of children registered as having a primary speech, language and communication need, lost this registration during transition to secondary education. We could not estimate the effect of SEN provision on academic outcomes, as the majority of children with high propensity to receive SEN provision did receive provision, and very few children in SEN settings completed statutory assessments.

Conclusions: Teacher perceptions of language difficulties at school entry, in the presence of additional risk factors, should prompt SEN provision. Recognition and support for language difficulties should be sustained throughout children's education.

Keywords: academic attainment; language disorder; school placement; special educational needs; statutory assessment.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared no competing or potential conflicts of interest.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Shows percentages of children that have a registered Special Educational Needs (SEN) in each academic year, split by whether or not they were identified as having language difficulties in Reception (age 4–5).
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Panel (A) shows percentage of children with a registered Special Educational Needs (SEN) in each primary need category in each year. Panel (B) shows the percentage of children with teacher identified language difficulties and a registered SEN in each primary need category in each year.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
An alluvium plot showing movement in and out of the Speech Language and Communication Need (SLCN) primary need category for all children that had ever appeared in this category between Reception (age 4–5) and Year 8 (age 12–13). Each bar shows number of children with SLCN listed as their primary need, those on the register with another primary need, and those who were not on the register in that year.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association, D. , & Association, A. P. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM‐5 (Vol. 5). American psychiatric association.
    1. Anders, Y. , Sammons, P. , Taggart, B. , Sylva, K. , Melhuish, E. , & Siraj‐Blatchford, I. (2010). The influence of child, family, home factors and pre‐school education on the identification of special educational needs at age 10. British Educational Research Journal, 37(3), 421–441. 10.1080/01411921003725338 - DOI
    1. Austin, P. C. (2011). An introduction to propensity score methods for reducing the effects of confounding in observational studies. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46(3), 399–424. 10.1080/00273171.2011.568786 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The children's communication checklist version 2 (CCC‐2). Psychological Corporation.
    1. Bishop, D. V. M. , Snowling, M. J. , Thompson, P. A. , & Greenhalgh, T. , & The Catalise Consortium . (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080. 10.1111/jcpp.12721 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources