Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Jun 16:13:RP96803.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.96803.

A common alteration in effort-based decision-making in apathy, anhedonia, and late circadian rhythm

Affiliations

A common alteration in effort-based decision-making in apathy, anhedonia, and late circadian rhythm

Sara Z Mehrhof et al. Elife. .

Abstract

Motivational deficits are common in several brain disorders, and motivational syndromes like apathy and anhedonia predict worse outcomes. Disrupted effort-based decision-making may represent a neurobiological underpinning of motivational deficits, shared across neuropsychiatric disorders. We measured effort-based decision-making in 994 participants using a gamified online task, combined with computational modelling, and validated offline for test-retest reliability. In two pre-registered studies, we first replicated studies linking impaired effort-based decision-making to neuropsychiatric syndromes, taking both a transdiagnostic and a diagnostic-criteria approach. Next, testing participants with early and late circadian rhythms in the morning and evening, we find circadian rhythm interacts with time-of-testing to produce parallel effects on effort-based decision-making. Circadian rhythm may be an important variable in computational psychiatry, decreasing reliability or distorting results when left unaccounted for. Disentangling effects of neuropsychiatric syndromes and circadian rhythm on effort-based decision-making will be essential to understand motivational pathologies and to develop tailored clinical interventions.

Keywords: circadian rhythm; computational biology; computational psychiatry; effort-based decision-making; human; motivational syndromes; neuroscience; systems biology.

Plain language summary

Our bodies are regulated by an internal circadian clock that aligns physiological processes to a 24-hour day-to-night cycle. However, the timing of this rhythm can vary: some people are ‘early birds’ who prefer mornings, while others are ‘night owls’ who prefer to wake up and stay up late. Circadian rhythms have been closely linked to neuropsychiatric conditions like depression, as well as specific psychiatric symptoms such as reduced motivation. Despite this, the circadian clock is seldom considered when investigating the cognitive and motivational changes associated with mental health conditions. To address this gap, Mehrhof and Nord designed a study to assess motivational differences in the general population and examine whether there were associations between neuropsychiatric symptoms and circadian rhythms. The study focused on effort-based decisions – where individuals choose whether completing a task is worth the effort of the reward – as disruptions in this process often underpin motivational deficits in neuropsychiatric disorders. Mehrhof and Nord found that individuals with high neuropsychiatric symptoms were less likely to undertake effort-based tasks, consistent with previous studies. Night owls showed the same motivational deficit – even when taking into account neuropsychiatric differences. However, this loss of motivation only occurred when the night owls were tested in the morning. When tested in the evening, their performance matched that of individuals who had an earlier circadian rhythm. These findings suggest that the circadian clock and neuropsychiatric conditions affect motivation in independent but parallel manners. In addition, testing someone at times of day that misalign with their circadian rhythm may be skewing the results of psychiatric studies. Further research could explore whether aligning treatment schedules and daily routines to a person’s internal clock improves motivation and other mental health outcomes.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

SM, CN No competing interests declared

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Correlations between questionnaire scores.
Correlations between questionnaire sum scores for the Snaith Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS), the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale (DARS), the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES), Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ), Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ), body mass index (BMI), and the Finish Diabetes Risk Score (FINDRISC) (n=958). Asterisks indicate significance: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 (not accounting for multiple comparisons). Note that sum scores for the AES and the DARS have been transformed such that increasing scores can be interpreted as higher symptom severity, in line with the SHAPS. Sum scores of the MEQ have been transformed such that higher scores indicate higher eveningness, in line with the MCTQ.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Effort-based decision-making: task design and model-agnostic results.
(A) The task can be divided into four phases: a calibration phase to determine individual clicking capacity to calibrate effort levels, practice trials that participants practice until successful on every effort level, instructions and a quiz that must be passed, and the main task, consisting of 64 trials split into 4 blocks. (B) Each trial consists of an offer with a reward (2, 3, 4, or 5 points) and an effort level (1, 2, 3, or 4, scaled to the required clicking speed and time the clicking must be sustained for) that subjects accept or reject. If accepted, a challenge at the respective effort level must be fulfilled for the required time to win the points. If rejected, subjects wait for a matched amount of time and receive one point. (C) Proportion of accepted trials, averaged across participants and effort–reward combinations. Error bars indicate standard errors (n = 958). (D) Staircasing development of offered effort and reward levels across the task, averaged across participants (n = 958).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Computational modelling: model visualization and model-based results.
(A) Economic decision-making models posit that efforts and rewards are joined into a subjective value (SV), weighed by individual effort (βE) and reward sensitivity (βR) parameters. The SV is then integrated with an acceptance bias parameter and translated to decision-making. (B, C) The model suggests that SV decreases as effort increases and increases as reward increases. The magnitude of this relationship depends on the individual effort and reward sensitivity parameters. (D) The acceptance bias parameter acts as an intercept to the softmax function, thereby changing the relationship between SV and acceptance probability. (E) Model comparison based on leave-out-out information criterion (LOOIC; lower is better) and expected log posterior density (ELPD; higher is better). Error bars indicate standard errors (n = 958). (F) Posterior predictive checks for the full parabolic model, comparing observed versus model-predicted subject-wise acceptance proportions across effort levels (left) and reward levels (right). Error bars indicate 95% highest density intervals (n = 958).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.. Associations between task parameter estimates and psychiatric measures.
(A) Visualizations of associations between the acceptance bias task parameter and the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS), the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale (DARS) (Rizvi et al., 2015), and the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) (Marin et al., 1991). (B, C) Comparison of acceptance bias (left) and effort sensitivity (right) between a sample of participants meeting criteria for current major depressive disorder (MDD; purple, upper) on the the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview 7.0.1 (M.I.N.I) (Lecrubier et al., 1997) and age- and gender-matched controls (yellow, lower).
Figure 5.
Figure 5.. Effects of chronotype and time-of-day on task parameter estimates.
(A) Effect of chronotype and time-of-day on reward sensitivity parameter estimates. (B) Effect of chronotype and time-of-day on acceptance bias parameter estimates.
Appendix 1—figure 1.
Appendix 1—figure 1.. Parameter recovery.
(A–C) Comparison between underlying parameters and recovered mean parameter estimates for the three free parameters of the full parabolic model. (D) Pearson’s correlations between all underlying and recovered parameters for the full parabolic model.
Appendix 1—figure 2.
Appendix 1—figure 2.. Parameter estimates.
(A–C) Visualization of individual-level (yellow) and group-level (blue) model parameter estimates for effort sensitivity (A), reward sensitivity (B), and acceptance bias (C).
Appendix 2—figure 1.
Appendix 2—figure 1.. Computational modelling and test–retest reliability.
(A) Model comparison for each testing session based on the leave-one-out information criterion (LOO) and expected log predictive density (ELPD). Error bars indicate standard errors (n = 30). (B) Subject-wise parameter estimates compared between testing sessions. (C) Predictive accuracy against chance (left) and group-level parameters (right; values >0 indicate better performance of subject-level compared to group-level parameters). Labels s1s2 (and s2s1) indicate session 1 (session 2) parameters predicting session 2 (session 1) data, s1s1 (and s2s2) indicate session 1 (session 2) parameters predicting session 1 (session 2) data.
Appendix 3—figure 1.
Appendix 3—figure 1.. Model-agnostic task measures relation to anhedonia.
(A) Comparing the proportion of accepted trials across effort (right) and reward (left) levels in subsamples of participants scoring in the highest and lowest SHAPS quartile. Error bars indicate standard errors (n = 479). (B) Distribution of effort–reward combinations, averaged across the final trial of 16 staircases.

Update of

  • doi: 10.31234/osf.io/z69w4
  • doi: 10.7554/eLife.96803.1
  • doi: 10.7554/eLife.96803.2
  • doi: 10.7554/eLife.96803.3

Similar articles

References

    1. Ahn WY, Krawitz A, Kim W, Busmeyer JR, Brown JW. A Model-Based fMRI analysis with hierarchical bayesian parameter estimation. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics. 2011;4:95–110. doi: 10.1037/a0020684. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ahn WY, Haines N, Zhang L. Revealing neurocomputational mechanisms of reinforcement learning and decision-making With the hBayesDM Package. Computational Psychiatry. 2017;1:24–57. doi: 10.1162/CPSY_a_00002. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ashton A, Jagannath A. Disrupted sleep and circadian rhythms in schizophrenia and their interaction with dopamine signaling. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2020;14:636. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00636. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Au J, Reece J. The relationship between chronotype and depressive symptoms: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2017;218:93–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.021. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bailey SL, Heitkemper MM. Circadian rhythmicity of cortisol and body temperature: morningness-eveningness effects. Chronobiology International. 2001;18:249–261. doi: 10.1081/cbi-100103189. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources