Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Jun 19;9(7):107488.
doi: 10.1016/j.cdnut.2025.107488. eCollection 2025 Jul.

Diet Patterns Featuring Western-Style and Low-Preparation Foods Differentially Relate to Cognitive Function in Early Childhood From the STRONG Kids 2 Birth Cohort Study

Affiliations

Diet Patterns Featuring Western-Style and Low-Preparation Foods Differentially Relate to Cognitive Function in Early Childhood From the STRONG Kids 2 Birth Cohort Study

Arden L McMath et al. Curr Dev Nutr. .

Abstract

Background: Dietary intake in early life is implicated in cognitive development.

Objectives: This study aimed to investigate how diet patterns derived at 2, 3 and 4 y old relate to executive functions and early cognitive and academic skills using data from the longitudinal STRONG Kids 2 cohort.

Methods: The Behavioral Inventory of Executive Functions Preschool caregiver survey was used to assess executive functions in 2 and 4 y olds. A subsample of children completed a modified Eriksen flanker to measure attentional inhibition, a hearts and flowers switch task to assess cognitive flexibility, and the Woodcock Johnson Early Cognitive and Academic Development tests to assess academic abilities during preschool ages (between 4 and 6 y old). Block Food Frequency Questionnaire items were grouped into 23 food groups, and dietary patterns were derived using principal component and confirmatory factor analyses. Three diet patterns were derived at each age (2, 3, and 4 y old); children were not assigned to a specific diet pattern but, rather, had 3 different diet pattern scores that were used for analyses. Diet pattern scores were used as predictors of executive functions at 2 (n = 217) and 4 y old (n = 250-266), as well as attentional inhibition (n = 53-56) and cognitive flexibility (n = 50-59) tasks and cognitive (n = 65-71) and academic (n = 55-57) scores in preschooler subsample.

Results: Diets with higher intake of processed meats, sweets, and fried foods at 3 y old was related to lower Woodcock Johnson scores (all β ≥ -0.351, false discovery rate-adjusted P value = 0.028), whereas those with higher intakes of grains, nuts/seeds, and condiments at 2 y was related to greater incongruent flanker accuracy (β: 0.380; ΔR 2 = 0.132; false discovery rate-adjusted P value = 0.030). Diet patterns from 2 to 4 y old were related to academic achievement and attentional inhibition; however, these associations were not independent of diet pattern at time of cognitive assessments.

Conclusions: This study emphasizes the potential value of exploring early diet interventions aimed at improving dietary patterns to support cognitive development.

Keywords: academic achievement; diet patterns; executive function; toddler.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

NAK and SMD report financial support was provided by General Mills, Inc. The authors report no conflicts of interest. Study sponsors had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, writing of the report, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Radar plots of factor loadings for confirmatory factor analysis–derived diet patterns at age of 2, 3, and 4 y. Note, a highly positive or negative factor loading indicates that the pattern is characterized by high intakes or low intakes of a food group, respectively. Diet patterns are named by their highest 3 factor loadings across the ages. (A) 2- and 3-y grains, nuts/seeds, and condiments pattern (GNC) and 4-y refined carbohydrates, grains, and other dairy pattern (RGD). (B) 2-, 3-, and 4-y processed meats, sweet and fried foods pattern (PSF). (C) 2-, 3-, and 4-y vegetables, starchy foods, and legumes pattern (VSL).

Similar articles

References

    1. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Moffitt T.E., Arseneault L., Belsky D., Dickson N., Hancox R.J., Harrington H., et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. U.S.A. 2011;108:2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Kadosh K.C., Muhardi L., Parikh P., Basso M., Mohamed H.J.J., Prawitasari T., et al. Nutritional support of neurodevelopment and cognitive function in infants and young children—an update and novel insights. Nutrients. 2021;13:199. doi: 10.3390/nu13010199. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. United States Department of Agriculture . USDA; Washington, DC: 2020. United States Department of Health and Human Services, Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2020-2025.
    1. Marinho A.R., Severo M., Vilela S., Costa R., de Lauzon-Guillain B., Torres D., et al. Is the association between dietary patterns and cognition mediated by children’s adiposity? A longitudinal approach in Generation XXI Birth Cohort. Clin. Nutr. 2022;41:231–237. doi: 10.1016/J.CLNU.2021.11.029. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources