Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Jul 21:16:1564963.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1564963. eCollection 2025.

Chess classes and executive function skills in 5-6 years old children: evidence from cross-sectional study

Affiliations

Chess classes and executive function skills in 5-6 years old children: evidence from cross-sectional study

Anastasia Yakushina et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

The aim of this study was to compare executive function skills in preschool children who were engaged in chess classes with those who were not. The participants were 88 typically developing 5-6-year-old children. There were two groups with 30 boys and 14 girls in each group: a group of chess players and a group of non-chess players. The results indicated that children who participated in chess classes exhibited significantly higher visuospatial working memory scores compared to their non-chess playing counterparts (U = 731, p = 0.05). Moreover, the analysis revealed no significant differences in the characteristics of extracurricular activities undertaken by the two groups. Consequently, the enhanced visuospatial working memory levels observed in the chess-playing group may be due to their participation in chess classes. Taken together, these findings suggest that chess classes may represent a viable method for enhancing visuospatial working memory in preschool-aged children.

Keywords: chess classes; cognitive development; executive function skills; extracurricular activities; preschool children.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Similar articles

References

    1. Addarii F., Ciracò E., Polo K., Neri E. (2022). Chess at school, executive functions and pandemic: an exploratory study. Quaderni ACP 29, 153–156. doi: 10.53141/QACP.2022.153-156 - DOI
    1. Atherton M., Zhuang J., Bart W. M., Hu X., He S. (2003). A functional MRI study of high-level cognition. i. the game of chess. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 26–31. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00207-0 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bang H., Won D., Park S. (2020). School engagement, self-esteem, and depression of adolescents: the role of sport participation and volunteering activity and gender differences. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 113:105012. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105012 - DOI
    1. Bayanova L., Chichinina E., Aslanova M. (2024). The association between music training and executive function in 6–7-year-old children. Front. Educ. 9:1333580. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1333580 - DOI
    1. Bergman Nutley S., Bohlin G., Thorell L. B., Klingberg T., Lindqvist S. (2008). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 12, 106–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources