Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Aug 14;15(1):29891.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-025-13704-x.

Space- and object-based attention in patients with a single hemisphere following childhood resection

Affiliations

Space- and object-based attention in patients with a single hemisphere following childhood resection

Sophia Robert et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

The neural processes underlying attentional processing are typically lateralized in adults, with spatial attention associated with the right hemisphere (RH) and object-based attention with the left hemisphere (LH). Using a modified two-rectangle attention paradigm, we compared the lateralization profiles of individuals with childhood hemispherectomy (either LH or RH) and age-matched, typically developing controls. Although patients exhibited slower reaction times (RTs) compared to controls, both groups benefited from valid attentional cueing. However, patients experienced significantly higher costs for invalid trials-reflected by larger RT differences between validly and invalidly cued targets. Notably, controls showed no significant RT cost differences between invalidly cued locations on cued versus uncued objects. By contrast, patients, irrespective of which hemisphere was resected, exhibited greater RT costs for targets on uncued versus cued objects, suggesting greater difficulty shifting attention across objects. We explore potential explanations for this group difference and the lack of difference between patients with LH or RH resection. These findings enhance our understanding of spatial- and object-based attention in typical development and reveal how significant neural injury affects the development of attentional systems in the LH and RH.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Declarations. Competing interests: Behrmann is a co-founder of and holds equity in Precision Neuroscopics. All other authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
The two-rectangle attention paradigm of Egly et al.. Two rectangles appear on the screen, followed by an ‘exogenous’ or bottom-up cue (black outline), in one of four locations. After a brief interstimulus interval (ISI), usually 100–150 ms, the target (black square) appears at one end of one rectangle in one of three possible locations (it never occurs on the diagonal opposite the cued corner). In Valid (V) trials (first row), the target appears in the same location and object (rectangle) as the cue—this occurs on 70–75% of the trials, leading the observer to use the cue to predict target location. In the Invalid Spatial (IS) trials (second row), following the cue, the target appears at a different location, at the same relative position on the other, uncued object. In the Invalid Object (IO) trials (third row), following the cue, the target appears at an uncued location that is spatially equidistant from the cue as in IS trials but is within the same cued object. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible to the presence of the target. After a response window of 2000 ms, the next trial was presented after an inter-trial interval (ITI).
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Two-rectangle attention paradigm modified to accommodate hemianopia in hemispheric surgery patients. Participants were instructed to fixate on ‘Doug the Dog’ and help him decide which colored ball (red or purple) appeared on his bones at any given time by pressing one key if they saw the red ball and another key if they saw the purple ball. They were not informed about the presence of the pre-cue that flashed in either a valid location (70% of trials), an invalid spatial location on the opposite object of the target to be presented (10% of trials, ‘IS’), an invalid location on the same object of the target to be presented (but equidistant to the cue to target distance of IS trials; 10% of trials, ‘IO’), or in all locations (neutral trials: 10%). After the cue appeared and a jittered interstimulus interval (between 100–150 ms) passed, participants had 2000 ms to identify the color of the target. A happy ‘Doug’ was shown if a response was completed in the allotted time irrespective of accuracy, and a sad ‘Doug’ was shown if no response was completed.
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Distribution of reaction times for patients (red) and controls (blue) for each trial type, respectively. The line in each box represents the median, while the lower and upper ranges represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively; the whiskers extend to 1.5 times the interquartile range. Stimuli were only presented to patients’ intact hemifield, i.e., the hemifield opposite to their intact hemisphere. Thus, LH surgery patients saw stimuli in their left hemifield (circles), and RH surgery patients saw stimuli in their right hemifield (triangles). The same was the case for patients’ matched controls. We included statistical details in the main text.
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Post hoc comparisons of estimated marginal mean reaction times for the validity effect (N-V), the object-based attention effect (IS-IO), and spatial attention effects (IS-V and IS-N) for controls (blue), patients (red), and across groups (gray). Error bars represent standard error. * denotes p-value < 0.05 after false discovery rate correction. See Table S6 for values.

Update of

References

    1. Eckstein, M. P. Probabilistic computations for attention, eye movements, and search. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci.3, 319–342 (2017). - PubMed
    1. Wolfe, J. M. Visual search: How do we find what we are looking for?? Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci.6, 539–562 (2020). - PubMed
    1. Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P. & Shulman, G. L. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci.3, 292–297 (2000). - PubMed
    1. Geng, J. J. & Behrmann, M. Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search. Percept. Psychophys.67, 1252–1268 (2005). - PubMed
    1. Posner, M. I. Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.32, 3–25 (1980). - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources