Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Aug;21(8):20250179.
doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2025.0179. Epub 2025 Aug 20.

Wild jackdaws learn to tolerate juveniles to exploit new foraging opportunities

Affiliations

Wild jackdaws learn to tolerate juveniles to exploit new foraging opportunities

Josh J Arbon et al. Biol Lett. 2025 Aug.

Abstract

Social tolerance can enhance access to resources and is thought to be crucial in facilitating the evolution of cooperation, social cognition and culture, but it is unknown whether animals can optimize their social tolerance through learning. We presented wild jackdaws (Corvus monedula) with a novel social information problem using automated feeders. Juveniles could always feed (simulating a situation where juveniles were sources of information about a new resource) but adults could only access food if they inhibited their tendency to displace juveniles and instead showed tolerance by occupying an adjacent feeder perch. Accordingly, adults learned to tolerate juveniles, with some evidence they generalized across juveniles as a cohort. The ability to learn to tolerate sources of valuable information, and generalize across cohorts of informed individuals, may facilitate adaptive responses in the face of environmental change and help to explain the success of jackdaws in human-dominated environments.

Keywords: aggression; flexibility; generalization; information use; learning; social; social information; social structure; tolerance.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

We declare we have no competing interests.

Figures

(a) Experimental array, showing an adult (right) in a successful co-occupation with a juvenile
Figure 1.
(a) Experimental array, showing an adult (right) in a successful co-occupation with a juvenile. RFID tags (lower circle), determine access to mealworms (upper circle) by motor-controlled doors. (b) The daily proportion of visits where adults successfully co-occupied experimental arrays with, or displaced, a juvenile.
The difference in likelihood of an adult (a) co-occupation with, and (b) displacing a juvenile
Figure 2.
The difference in likelihood of an adult (a) co-occupation with, and (b) displacing a juvenile (orange) as opposed to another adult (blue) as adults acquired experience of joining co-occupations with juveniles. Lines and shaded areas represent median and 95% REM estimates.

Similar articles

References

    1. Cronin KA, Sánchez A. 2012. Social dynamics and cooperation: the case of nonhuman primates and its implications for human behavior. Adv. Complex Syst. 15, 1250066. ( 10.1142/s021952591250066x) - DOI
    1. Massen JJM, Ritter C, Bugnyar T. 2015. Tolerance and reward equity predict cooperation in ravens (Corvus corax). Sci. Rep. 5, 1–11. ( 10.1038/srep15021) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Coelho CG, Garcia-Nisa I, Ottoni EB, Kendal RL. 2024. Social tolerance and success-biased social learning underlie the cultural transmission of an induced extractive foraging tradition in a wild tool-using primate. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 121, e2322884121. ( 10.1073/pnas.2322884121) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Cieri RL, Churchill SE, Franciscus RG, Tan J, Hare B. 2014. Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. Curr. Anthropol. 55, 419–443. ( 10.1086/677209) - DOI
    1. Testard C, et al. 2024. Ecological disturbance alters the adaptive benefits of social ties. Science 384, 1330–1335. ( 10.1126/science.adk0606) - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources