Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2025 Aug 29;20(8):e0330222.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0330222. eCollection 2025.

Diagnostic status influences rapport and communicative behaviours in dyadic interactions between autistic and non-autistic people

Affiliations

Diagnostic status influences rapport and communicative behaviours in dyadic interactions between autistic and non-autistic people

Themis Nikolas Efthimiou et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

A growing body of research suggests that the behaviours and experiences of autistic and non-autistic people are influenced by whether they are interacting with someone of the same or different diagnostic status. However, little is known about the relationship between these behaviours and the experiences of rapport in matched and mixed neurotype dyads. Using the Actor-Partner Interdependence Model, our pre-registered analyses examine how participants' and their partners' diagnostic statuses influence linguistic, behavioural, and kinematic indices, and how these relate to feelings of rapport among autistic and (n = 57; 17 self-diagnosed) non-autistic (n = 51) participants interacting within autistic (n = 20), non-autistic (n = 17), and mixed autistic-non-autistic (n = 17) dyads. We found that autistic participants reported lower rapport regardless of their partner's diagnostic status, though awareness of their partner's diagnostic status had a moderating effect. We observed a linguistic difference, autistic participants produced longer mean utterance lengths, these and other behavioural or kinematic indices did not mediate the relationship between diagnostic status and rapport across neurotypes. The current work highlights the need for a nuanced understanding of communication dynamics in autism.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Study setup for dyadic social interaction.
Note. Example layout of the study setup for the two members of a single dyad engaged in a social interaction. The image illustrates the spatial arrangement and environment used in the study, with participants’ faces blurred out to ensure anonymity.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Visualisation of the total rapport scores for actors across diagnostic statuses.
Note. The half-violin plots show the distribution of total rapport scores, overlaid with boxplots summarising the median and interquartile ranges, and jittered points representing individual data. The grey diamond indicates the mean for each diagnostic group. Non-autistic and autistic diagnostic statuses are shown, highlighting differences in rapport across actor roles.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Dyad neurotype and partner’s diagnostic status affect rapport.
Note. Transparent data points represent individual observations, and solid points and lines represent model-predicted averages with 95% confidence intervals.

Similar articles

References

    1. Hagad JL, Legaspi R, Numao M, Suarez M. Predicting levels of rapport in dyadic interactions through automatic detection of posture and posture congruence. In: 2011 IEEE Third Int’l Conference on Privacy, Security, Risk and Trust and 2011 IEEE Third Int’l Conference on Social Computing. 2011. pp. 613–6. doi: 10.1109/passat/socialcom.2011.143 - DOI
    1. Puccinelli NM, Tickle-Degnen L, Rosenthal R. Effect of dyadic context on judgments of rapport: Dyad task and partner presence. J Nonverbal Behav. 2023;2.
    1. Alsarrani A, Hunter RF, Dunne L, Garcia L. Association between friendship quality and subjective wellbeing among adolescents: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):2420. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-14776-4 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Hall JA, Horgan TG, Murphy NA. Nonverbal communication. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:271–94. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103145 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Orben A, Tomova L, Blakemore S-J. The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental health. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2020;4(8):634–40. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30186-3 - DOI - PMC - PubMed