Similar attentional, frequency, and associative effects for pseudohomophones and words
- PMID: 8440983
- DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.19.1.166
Similar attentional, frequency, and associative effects for pseudohomophones and words
Abstract
Between the presentation and recall of 1 or 5 digits, Ss performed a secondary task of naming a visually presented letter string--a pseudohomophone (e.g., FOLE, HOAP) or its real-word counterpart (FOAL, HOPE). Memory load interacted with frequency (HOPE vs. FOAL, HOAP vs. FOLE) but not with lexicality (HOPE vs. HOAP, FOAL vs. FOLE). This outcome counters models in which nonwords are named by a slow (resource-expensive) process that assembles phonology and words are named by a fast (resource-inexpensive) process that accesses lexical phonology. When the associative priming-of-naming task was secondary to the memory task, pseudohomophone associative priming (HOAP-DESPAIR, FOLE-HORSE) equaled associative priming (HOPE-DESPAIR, FOAL-HORSE) and was affected in the same way by memory load. Assembled phonology seems to underlie the naming of both words and nonwords.
Similar articles
-
Strategic effects in associative priming with words, homophones, and pseudohomophones.J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002 Sep;28(5):951-61. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002. PMID: 12219801
-
Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 1. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones.J Exp Psychol Gen. 1994 Jun;123(2):107-28. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.123.2.107. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1994. PMID: 8014609
-
Phonological access of the lexicon: evidence from associative priming with pseudohomophones.J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1991 Nov;17(4):951-66. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.17.4.951. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1991. PMID: 1837306
-
Being forward not backward: lexical limits to masked priming.Cognition. 2008 May;107(2):673-84. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.016. Epub 2007 Sep 4. Cognition. 2008. PMID: 17765887
-
Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review.Psychon Bull Rev. 2003 Dec;10(4):785-813. doi: 10.3758/bf03196544. Psychon Bull Rev. 2003. PMID: 15000531 Review.
Cited by
-
Further evidence for phonological constraints on visual lexical access: TOWED primes FROG.Percept Psychophys. 1993 May;53(5):461-6. doi: 10.3758/bf03205193. Percept Psychophys. 1993. PMID: 8332414
-
Diagnostics of phonological lexical processing: pseudohomophone naming advantages, disadvantages, and base-word frequency effects.Mem Cognit. 2002 Sep;30(6):969-87. doi: 10.3758/bf03195781. Mem Cognit. 2002. PMID: 12450099
-
When do nonwords activate semantics? Implications for models of visual word recognition.Mem Cognit. 1998 Jan;26(1):61-74. doi: 10.3758/bf03211370. Mem Cognit. 1998. PMID: 9519697
-
Testing the role of phonology in reading: focus on sentence processing.J Psycholinguist Res. 2009 Aug;38(4):333-44. doi: 10.1007/s10936-008-9092-0. Epub 2008 Dec 2. J Psycholinguist Res. 2009. PMID: 19048378
-
Effects of association, frequency, and stimulus quality on naming words in the presence or absence of pseudowords.Mem Cognit. 1995 May;23(3):289-300. doi: 10.3758/bf03197231. Mem Cognit. 1995. PMID: 7791598
Publication types
MeSH terms
Grants and funding
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources