Ear differences in evaluating emotional tones of voice and verbal content
- PMID: 845557
- DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.3.1.75
Ear differences in evaluating emotional tones of voice and verbal content
Abstract
Two experiments are presented in which lateralization of monaural, auditory input affected the evaluation of verbal passages. In Experiment 1, subjects listened to taped passages that consisted of three levels (positive, neutral, and negative) of tone of voice crossed with three similar levels of content or meaning, making a total of nine different tone/content combinations. Subjects rated these passages as positive, neutral, or negative, and although they were not instructed about which cues (tone or content) to use, 29 of 36 subjects who listened on the left ear primarily used the tone-of-voice cues to rate the passages, whereas 21 of 36 subjects who listened on the right ear used the content cues (p less than .01). In Experiment 2, subjects were asked to make objective ratings of both tone of voice and content. Subjects who listened on the right ear were more accurate in rating both cues. The lateral differences in evaluating the auditory stimuli are discussed in the framework of the left hemisphere's specialization for analytic processing of objective information and the right hemisphere's specialization for the holistic processing of subjective or emotional information. However, each hemisphere's specialization for tone and content cues can be greatly affected by the demands of the experimental task.
Similar articles
-
The interaction of hemispheric preference, laterality, and sex in the perception of emotional tone and verbal content.Int J Neurosci. 1981;13(2-3):121-6. doi: 10.3109/00207458109043311. Int J Neurosci. 1981. PMID: 7239783
-
Children below 9 years use both verbal cues and lateralized cues to orient their attention in an emotional dichotic listening task.Laterality. 2014;19(5):533-48. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.868470. Epub 2013 Dec 20. Laterality. 2014. PMID: 24359279
-
Ear asymmetry in perception of emotional non-verbal stimuli.Acta Psychol (Amst). 1973 Dec;37(6):351-7. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(73)90002-4. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1973. PMID: 4767606 No abstract available.
-
Simultaneous verbal and affective laterality effects.Neuropsychologia. 1994 Jul;32(7):787-97. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90017-5. Neuropsychologia. 1994. PMID: 7936162
-
Auditory evoked potentials during multichannel selective listening: role of pitch and localization cues.J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1976 Aug;2(3):313-25. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.2.3.313. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1976. PMID: 993738
Cited by
-
Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional tone of voice.Mem Cognit. 2002 Jun;30(4):583-93. doi: 10.3758/bf03194959. Mem Cognit. 2002. PMID: 12184559
-
Intentionality and "free-will" from a neurodevelopmental perspective.Front Integr Neurosci. 2012 Jun 27;6:36. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00036. eCollection 2012. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012. PMID: 22754510 Free PMC article.
-
A conditioned response model of the placebo effect predictions from the model.Biofeedback Self Regul. 1980 Mar;5(1):5-18. doi: 10.1007/BF00999060. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1980. PMID: 7370359
-
Asymmetry in the relationship between finger temperature changes and emotional state in males.Biofeedback Self Regul. 1989 Dec;14(4):281-90. doi: 10.1007/BF00999119. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1989. PMID: 2631969
-
Excitability of motor cortices as a function of emotional sounds.PLoS One. 2013 May 7;8(5):e63060. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063060. Print 2013. PLoS One. 2013. PMID: 23667574 Free PMC article.
Publication types
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources