Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 1998 Sep;34(5):982-95.
doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.982.

Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior

Affiliations

Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior

J E Bates et al. Dev Psychol. 1998 Sep.

Abstract

Child temperament and parental control were studied as interacting predictors of behavior outcomes in 2 longitudinal samples. In Sample 1, data were ratings of resistant temperament and observed restrictive control in infancy-toddlerhood and ratings of externalizing behavior at ages 7 to 10 years; in Sample 2, data were retrospective ratings of temperament in infancy-toddlerhood, observed restrictive control at age 5 years, and ratings of externalizing behavior at ages 7 to 11 years. Resistance more strongly related to externalizing in low-restriction groups than in high-restriction groups. This was true in both samples and for both teacher- and mother-rated outcomes. Several Temperament x Environment interaction effects have been reported previously, but this is one of very few replicated effects.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Scatter plots for the Bloomington Longitudinal Study (BLS) sample of temperamental resistance to control predicting externalizing outcomes as reported by teachers (top panel) and mothers (bottom panel). High Restrict = high restrictive control (for teachers, r = .09, ns; for mothers, r = .11. ns); Low Restrict = low restrictive control (for teachers, r = .27, p = .05; for mothers, r = .44, p= .001); Resist Control = child resistance to control; ICQ = Infant Characteristics Questionnaire.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Scatter plots for the Child Development Project (CDP) sample of temperamental resistance to control predicting externalizing outcomes as reported by teachers (top panel) and mothers (bottom panel). High Restrict = high restrictive control (for teachers, r = .01, ns; for mothers, r = .21, p = .13); Low Restrict = low restrictive control (for teachers, r = .22, p = .04; for mothers, r = .53, p = .000); RICQ = Retrospective Infant Characteristics Questionnaire.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Basic design of multiple-group, structural model (repeated four times, for teacher- and mother-report outcomes and for Bloomington Longitudinal Study and Child Development Project data sets). EXT = eternalizing behavior score.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont. Department of Psychiatry; 1991a.
    1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991b.
    1. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1983.
    1. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form and Teacher Version of the Child Behavior Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1986.
    1. Arcus D, Gardner S. When biology is not destiny; Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; New Orleans, LA. 1993. Mar,

Publication types